Skip to main content
Free AccessOriginalarbeit

Abrufeffekte im Gedächtnis: Ein Überblick zur aktuellen Grundlagenforschung

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0033-3042/a000517

Zusammenfassung. Der Frage, wie Erinnern das Gedächtnis formt, wurde in der Kognitiven Psychologie in letzter Zeit große Aufmerksamkeit gewidmet. Testungseffekte, die in einer durch Gedächtnisabruf in der Folge verbesserten Zugänglichkeit von Gedächtniseinträgen bestehen, wurden in diesem Zusammenhang insbesondere auch hinsichtlich ihres pädagogischen Potentials diskutiert. Neben erleichterter Zugänglichkeit kann Gedächtnisabruf allerdings auch Vergessen nicht abgerufener Information verursachen. Der aktuelle Stand der Grundlagenforschung zu Abrufeffekten wird in diesem Überblicksartikel dargestellt und eine integrative Betrachtung unterschiedlicher Arten von Abrufeffekten unter Berücksichtigung wichtiger Moderatorvariablen versucht.


Retrieval Effects on Memory: Overview of Current Basic Research

Abstract. The question as to how retrieval shapes memory has received much attention in cognitive psychology recently. In particular, testing effects consisting of enhanced accessibility of memory representations as a consequence of memory retrieval have been discussed also with regard to their educational potential. However, besides enhanced accessibility, memory retrieval also can cause forgetting of non-retrieved information. Here, we describe the present state of basic research on retrieval effects and try to integrate different kinds of retrieval effects, with a special consideration of important moderator variables.

Literatur

  • Adesope, O. O., Trevisan, D. A. & Sundararajan, N. (2017). Rethinking the use of tests: A meta-analysis of practice testing. Review of Educational Research, 87, 659 – 701. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654316689306 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Agarwal, P. K., Finley, J. R., Rose, N. S. & Roediger, H. L. (2017). Benefits from retrieval practice are greater for students with lower working memory capacity. Memory, 25, 764 – 77. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1220579 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanisms of forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 415 – 445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2003.08.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. & Bell, T. (2001). Forgetting our facts: The role of inhibitory processes in the loss of propositional knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 544 – 570. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.544 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C., Bjork, R. A. & Bjork, E. L. (1994). Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1063 – 1087. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, M. C. & McCulloch, K. C. (1999). Integration as a general boundary condition on retrieval-induced forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 608 – 629. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.25.3.608 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Arnold, K. M. & McDermott, K. B. (2013). Test-potentiated learning: Distinguishing between direct and indirect effects of tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 940 – 945. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029199 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aslan, A. & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2016). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older but not in younger elementary school children. Developmental Science, 19, 992 – 998. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12340 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Balota, D. A., Duchek, J. M., Sergent-Marshall, S. D. & Roediger, H. L. III. (2006). Does expanded retrieval produce benefits over equal-interval spacing? Explorations of spacing effects in healthy aging and early stage Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 21, 19 – 31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.19 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barenberg, J. & Dutke, S. (2019). Testing and metacognition: Retrieval practise effects on metacognitive monitoring in learning from text. Memory, 27 (3), 269 – 279. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2018.1506481 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barnier, A. J., Hung, L. & Conway, M. A. (2004). Retrieval-induced forgetting of emotional and unemotional autobiographical memories. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 457 – 477. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269993034000392 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bäuml, K.-H. (2002). Semantic generation can cause episodic forgetting. Psychological Science, 13, 357 – 361. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00464 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bäuml, K.-H. (2008). Inhibitory processes. In H. L. RoedigerIII (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory. Vol. 2 of Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (pp. 195 – 220). Oxford: Elsevier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bäuml, K.-H. T. & Kliegl, O. (2013). The critical role of retrieval processes in release from proactive interference. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 39 – 53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.07.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bertilsson, F., Wiklund-Hörnqvist, C., Stenlund, T. & Jonsson, B. (2017). The testing effect and its relation to working memory capacity and personality characteristics. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 16, 241 – 259. https://doi.org/10.1891/1945-8959.16.3.241 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings. In J. Metcalfe und A. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 185 – 205). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, G. A. & Unsworth, N. (2012). Individual differences in the effects of retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 407 – 415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.12.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Butler, A. C. (2010). Repeated Testing Produces Superior Transfer of Learning Relative to Repeated Studying. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1118 – 1133. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019902 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Butler, A. C., Karpicke, J. D. & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The effect of type and timing of feedback on learning from multiple-choice tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 13, 273 – 281. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898x.13.4.273 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, S. K. (2009). Cue strength as a moderator of the testing effect: The benefits of elaborative retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1563 – 1569. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017021 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, S. K. & DeLosh, E. L. (2006). Impoverished cue support enhances subsequent retention: support for the elaborative retrieval explanation of the testing effect. Memory and Cognition, 34, 268 – 276. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193405 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, S. K., Lund, T. J. S., Coffman, C. R., Armstrong, P. I., Lamm, M. H. & Reason, R. D. (2016). A classroom study on the relationship between student achievement and retrieval-enhanced learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 353 – 375. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9311-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, J. C. K. (2009). When does retrieval induce forgetting and when does it induce facilitation? Implications for retrieval inhibition, testing effect, and text processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 153 – 170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.04.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, J. C. K., Erdman, M. R. & Davis, S. D. (2015). Retrieval induces forgetting, but only when nontested items compete for retrieval: Implication for interference, inhibition, and context reinstatement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1298 – 1315. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, J. C. K., Manley, K. D., Davis, S. D. & Szpunar, K. K. (2018). Testing potentiates new learning across a retention interval and a lag: A strategy change perspective. Journal of Memory and Language, 102, 83 – 96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.05.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, J. C. K., McDermott, K. B. & Roediger, H. L., III. (2006). Retrieval-induced facilitation: Initially nontested material can benefit from prior testing of related material. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 553 – 571. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.4.553 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cho, K. W., Neely, J. H., Crocco, S. & Vitrano, D. (2017). Testing enhances both encoding and retrieval for both tested and untested items. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 1211 – 1235. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1175485 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coane, J. H. (2013). Retrieval practice and elaborative encoding benefit memory in younger and older adults. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2, 95 – 100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2013.04.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Congleton, A. & Rajaram, S. (2012). The origin of the interaction between learning method and delay in the testing effect: The roles of processing and conceptual retrieval organization. Memory & Cognition, 40, 528 – 539. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0168-y First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dawans, B. von, Kirschbaum, C. & Heinrichs, M. (2011). The Trier Social Stress Test for Groups (TSST-G): A new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress exposure in a group format. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 514 – 522. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., Nolan, D. & Singleton, J. (2007). Expanding retrieval practice: An effective aid to preschool children’s learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 991 – 1004. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210600823595 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hauer, B. J. A. & Wessel, I. (2006). Retrieval-induced forgetting of autobiographical memory details. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 430 – 447. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500342464 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jaeger, A., Eisenkraemer, R. E. & Stein, L. M. (2015). Test-enhanced learning in third-grade children. Educational Psychology, 35, 513 – 521. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2014.963030 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jönsson, F. U., Kubik, V., Sundqvist, M. L., Todorov, I. & Jonsson, B. (2014). How crucial is the response format for the testing effect? Psychological Research, 78, 623 – 633. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0522-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, A. C., Wardlow, L., Pan, S. C., Zepeda, C., Heyman, G. D., Dunlosky, J. & Rickard, T. C. (2016). Beyond the rainbow: Retrieval practice leads to better spelling than does rainbow writing. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 385 – 400. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9330-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karpicke, J. D. (2017). Retrieval-based learning: A decade of progress. In J. T. Wixted (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory, Vol. 2 of Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (J. H. Byrne, Series Ed., pp. 487 – 514). Oxford: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Karpicke, J. D. & Aue, W. R. (2015). The testing effect is alive and well with complex materials. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 317 – 326. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9309-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karpicke, J. D. & Roediger, H. L. (2008). The critical importance of retrieval for learning. Science, 319, 966 – 968. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1152408 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karpicke, J. D. & Zaromb, F. M. (2010). Retrieval mode distinguishes the testing effect from the generation effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 227 – 239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.11.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M. & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’ – a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76 – 81. https://doir.org/10.1159/000119004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kliegl, O., Abel, M. & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2018). A (preliminary) recipe for obtaining a testing effect in preschool children: two critical ingredients. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1446. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01446 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kornell, N., Bjork, R. A. & Garcia, M. A. (2011). Why tests appear to prevent forgetting: A distribution-based bifurcation model. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 85 – 97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.04.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuhbandner, C. & Emmerdinger, K. J. (2019). Do students really prefer repeated rereading over testing when studying textbooks? A reexamination. Memory, 27, 952 – 961. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2019.1610177 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kühn, A. (1914). Über Einprägung durch Lesen und durch Rezitieren. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 68, 396 – 481. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lipko-Speed, A., Dunlosky, J. & Rawson, K. A. (2014). Does testing with feedback help grade-school children learn key concepts in science? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3, 171 – 176. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.04.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lipowski, S. L., Pyc, M. A., Dunlosky, J. & Rawson, K. A. (2014). Establishing and explaining the testing effect in free recall for young children. Developmental Psychology, 50, 994 – 1000. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meyer, A. N. & Logan, J. M. (2013). Taking the testing effect beyond the college freshman: Benefits for lifelong learning. Psychology and Aging, 28, 142 – 147. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030890 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Minear, M., Coane, J. H., Boland, S. C., Cooney, L. H. & Albat, M. (2018). The benefits of retrieval practice depend on item difficulty and intelligence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 1474 – 1486. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000486 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Murayama, K., Miyatsu, T., Buchli, D. & Storm, B.C. (2014). Forgetting as a consequence of retrieval: A meta-analytic review of retrieval-induced forgetting. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1383 – 1409. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037505 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B. & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2014). Retrieval practice enhances new learning: the forward effect of testing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 286. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00286 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B. & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2018). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 34, 242 – 250. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000307 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., Engel, M. & Frings, C. (2018). The forward effect of testing: Behavioral evidence for the reset-of-encoding hypothesis using serial position analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1197. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01197 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B. & Frings, C. (2019). The forward testing effect is reliable and independent of learners’ working memory capacity. Journal of Cognition, 2, 37. https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.82 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., Schicker, S., Niedernhuber, J. & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2011). Retrieval during learning facilitates subsequent memory encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 287 – 297. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021801 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., von Dawans, B., Domes, G. & Frings, C. (2020). The forward testing effect is immune to acute psychosocial encoding/retrieval stress. Experimental Psychology, 67, 112 – 122. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000472 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., Weber, J. & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2013). Using testing to improve learning after severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 27, 280 – 285. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031797 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pyc, M. A. & Rawson, K. A. (2010). Why testing improves memory: Mediator effectiveness hypothesis. Science, 330, 335. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1191465 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Putnam, A. L. & Roediger, H. L. III. (2013). Does response mode affect amount recalled or the magnitude of the testing effect? Memory & Cognition, 41, 36 – 48. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0245-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rafidi, N. S., Hulbert, J. C., Brooks, P. P. & Norman, K. A. (2018). Reductions in retrieval competition predict the benefit of repeated testing. Scientific Reports, 8, 11714. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-29686-y First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roediger, H. L. III. & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 20 – 27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roediger, H. L. III. & Karpicke, J. D. (2006). The power of testing memory: Basic research and implications for educational practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 181 – 210. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00012.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rohrer, D., Taylor, K. & Sholar, B. (2010). Tests enhance the transfer of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 233 – 239. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017678 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rowland, C. A. (2014). The effect of testing versus restudy on retention: A meta-analytic review of the testing effect. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1432 – 1463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037559 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shaw, S., III, Bjork, A., R. & Handal, A. (1995). Retrieval-induced forgetting in an eyewitness-memory paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 249 – 253. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210965 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., McCullough, A. M. & Yonelinas, A. P. (2017). The effects of acute stress on episodic memory: a meta-analysis and integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 636 – 675. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000100 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slamecka, N. J. (1968). An examination of trace storage in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 504 – 513. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025695 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. D. (1971). Output interference and organized recall from long-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 400 – 408. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(71)80039-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. M., Davis, F. C. & Thomas, A. K. (2018). Criterial learning is not enough: Retrieval practice is necessary for improving post-stress memory accessibility. Behavioral neuroscience, 132, 161 – 170. https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000240 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. M., Floerke, V. A. & Thomas, A. K. (2016). Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress. Science, 354, 1046 – 1048. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aah5067 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. M., Roediger, H. L. III & Karpicke, J. D. (2013). Covert retrieval practice benefits retention as much as overt retrieval practice. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1712 – 1725. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033569 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Storm, B. C., Bjork, E. L. & Bjork, R. A. (2007). When intended remembering leads to unintended forgetting. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 909 – 915. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701288706 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Storm, B. C., Bjork, E. L., Bjork, R. A. & Nestojko, J. F. (2006). Is retrieval success a necessary condition for retrieval-induced forgetting? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 1023 – 1027. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213919 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sumowski, J. F., Wood, H. G., Chiaravalloti, N., Wylie, G. R., Lengenfelder, J. & DeLuca, J. (2010). Retrieval practice: a simple strategy for improving memory after traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 1147 – 1150. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710001128 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szőllősi, Á., Keresztes, A., Novák, B., Szászi, B., Kéri, S. & Racsmány, M. (2017). The testing effect is preserved in stressful final testing environment. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31, 615 – 622. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3363 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szpunar, K. K., McDermott, K. B. & Roediger, H. L., III. (2008). Testing during study insulates against the buildup of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1392 – 1399. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013082 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T., Aslan, A. & Frings, C. (2016). Competition dependence of retrieval-induced forgetting in motor memory. Memory & Cognition, 44, 671 – 680. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-015-0578-3/1037/a0030336 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Frings, C. (2013). Resolving interference between body movements: Retrieval-induced forgetting of motor sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1152 – 1161. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030336 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Frings, C. (2014). Forgetting motor programmes: Retrieval dynamics in procedural memory. Memory, 22, 1116 – 1125. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2013.871293 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Frings, C. (2015). Interference in episodic memory: Retrieval-induced forgetting of unknown words. Psychological Research, 79, 795 – 800. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-014-0604-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Frings, C. (2017). Retrieval-induced forgetting is retrieval-modality specific: Evidence from motor memory. Cognition, 162, 143 – 152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Frings, C. (2018). Feedback increases benefits but not costs of retrieval practice: Retrieval-induced forgetting is strength independent. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 636 – 642. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1450-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Frings, C. (2019). Testing enhances motor practice. Memory & Cognition, 47, 1270 – 1283. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00932-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T., Kaufmann, K., Kranz, J. & Möller, A. (2020). Retrieval-based skill learning: testing promotes the acquisition of scientific experimentation skills. Psychological Research, 84, 660 – 666. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1088-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Kubik, V. (2017). Test-potentiated learning of motor sequences. Memory, 25, 326 – 334. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2016.1171880 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T., Loran, I. & Frings, C. (2015). Dancing your moves away: How memory retrieval shapes complex motor action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 21, 300 – 312. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000052 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T., Niederée, C., Jilek, C., Ceroni, A., Maus, H., Runge, Y. & Frings, C. (2019). Temporarily unavailable: Memory inhibition in cognitive and computer science. Interacting with Computers, 31, 231 – 249. https://doi.org/10.1093/iwc/iwz013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T. & Wippich, W. (2012). The processing of inter-item relations as a moderating factor of retrieval-induced forgetting. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 8, 218 – 225. https://doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0117-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Toppino, T. C. & Cohen, M. S. (2009). The testing effect and the retention interval: questions and answers. Experimental Psychology, 56, 252 – 257. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.252 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Tse, C. S. & Pu, X. (2012). The effectiveness of test-enhanced learning depends on trait test anxiety and working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 253 – 264. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029190 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tullis, J. G., Finley, J. R. & Benjamin, A. S. (2013). Metacognition of the testing effect: Guiding learners to predict the benefits of retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 41, 429 – 442. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0274-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Gog, T., Kester, L., Dirkx, K., Hoogerheide, V., Boerboom, J. & Verkoeijen, P. P. J. L. (2015). Testing after worked example study does not enhance delayed problem-solving performance compared to restudy. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 265 – 289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9297-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Gog, T. & Sweller, J. (2015). Not new, but nearly forgotten: the testing effect decreases or even disappears as the complexity of learning materials increases. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 247 – 264. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9310-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wissman, K. T. & Rawson, K. A. (2018). Test-potentiated learning: Three independent replications, a disconfirmed hypothesis, and an unexpected boundary condition. Memory, 26, 406 – 414. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1350717 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wissman, K. T., Rawson, K. A. & Pyc, M. A. (2011). The interim test effect: testing prior material can facilitate the learning of new material. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1140 – 1147. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0140-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Witasek, S. (1907). Über Lesen und Rezitieren in ihren Beziehungen zum Gedächtnis. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 44, 161 – 185. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wong, S. S. H., Ng, G. J. P., Tempel, T. & Lim, S. W. H. (2019). Retrieval practice enhances analogical problem solving. Journal of Experimental Education, 87, 128 – 138. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2017.1409185 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yang, C., Potts, R. & Shanks, D. R. (2018). Enhancing learning and retrieval of new information: a review of the forward testing effect. npj Science of Learning, 3, 8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-018-0024-y First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zaromb, F. M. & Roediger, H. L. (2010). The testing effect in free recall is associated with enhanced organizational processes. Memory & Cognition, 38, 995 – 1008. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.8.995 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar