Skip to main content
Research Article

The Forward Testing Effect is Immune to Acute Psychosocial Encoding/Retrieval Stress

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000472

Abstract. The forward testing effect (FTE) refers to the finding that testing of previously studied information enhances memory for subsequently studied other information. Previous research demonstrated that the FTE is a robust phenomenon that generalizes across different materials and populations. The present study examined whether the FTE is robust under acute psychosocial encoding/retrieval stress. In each of two experimental conditions, participants studied three item lists in anticipation of final cumulative recall testing. In the testing condition, participants were tested immediately on lists 1 and 2, whereas in the restudy condition, they restudied lists 1 and 2. In both conditions, participants were tested immediately on list 3. Acute psychosocial stress was induced in participants prior to the encoding of item lists using the Trier social stress test for groups protocol. No stress was induced in a control group. Salivary cortisol, alpha amylase, and subjective stress were measured repeatedly to capture the biopsychological stress response. The results showed a significant FTE on list 3 recall, that is, testing of lists 1 and 2 enhanced the recall of list 3. No significant effect of stress on the FTE was observed, suggesting that the FTE is robust under acute psychosocial encoding/retrieval stress. The discussion provides suggestions for future research directions.

References

  • Arnold, K. M., & McDermott, K. B. (2013). Test-potentiated learning: Distinguishing between direct and indirect effects of tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 940–945. 10.1037/a0029199 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Aslan, A., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2016). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older but not in younger elementary school children. Developmental Science, 19, 992–998. 10.1111/desc.12340 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bäuml, K.-H. T., & Kliegl, O. (2013). The critical role of retrieval processes in release from proactive interference. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 39–53. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.07.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, J. C. K., Manley, K. D., Davis, S. D., & Szpunar, K. K. (2018). Testing potentiates new learning across a retention interval and a lag: A strategy change perspective. Journal of Memory and Language, 102, 83–96. 10.1016/j.jml.2018.05.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, J. C. K., Meissner, C. A., & Davis, S. D. (2018). Retrieval potentiates new learning: A theoretical and meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 1111–1146. 10.1037/bul0000166 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Cho, K. W., Neely, J. H., Crocco, S., & Vitrano, D. (2017). Testing enhances both encoding and retrieval for both tested and untested items. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70, 1211–1235. 10.1080/17470218.2016.1175485 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Cornelisse, S., Joëls, M., & Smeets, T. (2011). A randomized trial on mineralocorticoid receptor blockade in men: Effects on stress responses, selective attention, and memory. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 2720–2728. 10.1038/npp.2011.162 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Duyck, W., Desmet, T., Verbeke, L. P. C., & Brysbaert, M. (2004). WordGen: A tool for word selection and non-word generation in Dutch, German, English, and French. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 488–499. 10.3758/bf03195595 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. 10.3758/bf03193146 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Izawa, C. (1971). The test trial potentiating model. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 8, 200–224. 10.1016/0022-2496(71)90012-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Karpicke, J. D. (2017). Retrieval-based learning: A decade of progress. In J. T. Wixted (Ed.), Cognitive psychology of memory, Vol. 2 of Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (J. H. Byrne, Series Ed., pp. 487–514). Oxford, UK: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’– A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28, 76–81. 10.1159/000119004 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kornell, N., Bjork, R. A., & Garcia, M. A. (2011). Why tests appear to prevent forgetting: A distribution-based bifurcation model. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 85–97. 10.1016/j.jml.2011.04.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuhlmann, S., Piel, M., & Wolf, O. T. (2005). Impaired memory retrieval after psychosocial stress in healthy young men. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(11), 2977–2982. 10.1523/jneurosci.5139-04.2005 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Maheu, F. S., Collicutt, P., Kornik, R., Moszkowski, R., & Lupien, S. J. (2005). The perfect time to be stressed: A differential modulation of human memory by stress applied in the morning or in the afternoon. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 29, 1281–1288. 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.08.012 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2014). Retrieval practice enhances new learning: The forward effect of testing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 286. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00286 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B. & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2019). Testing enhances subsequent learning in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 34, 242–250. 10.1037/pag0000307 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., Engel, M., & Frings, C. (2018). The forward effect of testing: Behavioral evidence for the reset-of-encoding hypothesis using serial position analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1197. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01197 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., & Frings, C. (2019). The forward testing effect is reliable and independent of learners’ working memory capacity. Journal of Cognition, 2, 37. 10.5334/joc.82 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., Kliegl, O., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2012). List-method directed forgetting: The forget cue improves both encoding and retrieval of postcue information. Memory & Cognition, 40, 861–873. 10.3758/s13421-012-0206-4 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., Schicker, S., Niedernhuber, J., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2011). Retrieval during learning facilitates subsequent memory encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 37, 287–297. 10.1037/a0021801 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pastötter, B., Weber, J., & Bäuml, K.-H. T. (2013). Using testing to improve learning after severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 27, 280–285. 10.1037/a0031797 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, J. D., Jackson, E. D., Hoscheidt, S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W. J., & Nadel, L. (2007). Stress administered prior to encoding impairs neutral but enhances emotional long-term episodic memories. Learning & Memory, 14, 861–868. 10.1101/lm.743507 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., McCullough, A. M., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2017). The effects of acute stress on episodic memory: A meta-analysis and integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 636–675. 10.1037/bul0000100 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. M., Davis, F. C. & Thomas, A. K. (2018). Criterial learning is not enough: Retrieval practice is necessary for improving post-stress memory accessibility. Behavioral Neuroscience, 132, 161–170. 10.1037/bne0000240 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. M., Floerke, V. A. & Thomas, A. K. (2016). Retrieval practice protects memory against acute stress. Science, 354, 1046–1048. 10.1126/science.aah5067 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. M., Race, E., Davis, F. C., & Thomas, A. K. (2019). Retrieval practice improves item memory but not source memory in the context of stress. Brain and Cognition, 133, 24–32. 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.12.005 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. M., & Thomas, A. K. (2018). Reducing the consequences of acute stress on memory retrieval. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7, 219–229. 10.1016/j.jarmac.2017.09.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szőllősi, Á., Keresztes, A., Novák, B., Szászi, B., Kéri, S., & Racsmány, M. (2017). The testing effect is preserved in stressful final testing environment. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31, 615–622. 10.1002/acp.3363 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Szpunar, K. K., Khan, N. Y., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Interpolated memory tests reduce mind wandering and improve learning of online lectures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 6313–6317. 10.1073/pnas.1221764110 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Szpunar, K. K., McDermott, K. B., & Roediger, H. L. 3rd (2008). Testing during study insulates against the buildup of proactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1392–1399. 10.1037/a0013082 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Tempel, T., & Frings, C. (2019). Testing enhances motor practice. Memory & Cognition, 47, 1270–1283. 10.3758/s13421-019-00932-6 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Toppino, T. C., & Cohen, M. S. (2009). The testing effect and the retention interval: Questions and answers. Experimental Psychology, 56, 252–257. 10.1027/1618-3169.56.4.252 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • von Dawans, B., Fischbacher, U., Kirschbaum, C., Fehr, E., & Heinrichs, M. (2012). The social dimension of stress reactivity: Acute stress increases prosocial behavior in humans. Psychological Science, 23, 651–660. 10.1177/0956797611431576 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • von Dawans, B., Kirschbaum, C., & Heinrichs, M. (2011). The Trier Social Stress Test for Groups (TSST-G): A new research tool for controlled simultaneous social stress exposure in a group format. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 514–522. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.08.004 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wagenmakers, E.-J., Love, J., Marsman, M., Jamil, T., Ly, A., Verhagen, A. J., … Morey, R. D. (2018). Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25, 58–76. 10.3758/s13423-017-1323-7 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Weinstein, Y., Gilmore, A. W., Szpunar, K. K., & McDermott, K. B. (2014). The role of test expectancy in the build-up of proactive interference in long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 1039–1048. 10.1037/a0036164 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wiemers, U. S., Sauvage, M. M., Schoofs, D., Hamacher-Dang, T. C., & Wolf, O. T. (2013). What we remember from a stressful episode. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 2268–2277. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.04.015 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wiemers, U. S., Sauvage, M. M., & Wolf, O. T. (2014). Odors as effective retrieval cues for stressful episodes. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 112, 230–236. 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.10.004 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wissman, K. T., Rawson, K. A., & Pyc, M. A. (2011). The interim test effect: Testing prior material can facilitate the learning of new material. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 1140–1147. 10.3758/s13423-011-0140-7 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Yang, C., Chew, S.-J., Sun, B., & Shanks, D. R. (2019). The forward effects of testing transfer to different domains of learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111, 809–826. 10.1037/edu0000320 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yang, C., Potts, R., & Shanks, D. R. (2018). Enhancing learning and retrieval of new information: A review of the forward testing effect. NPJ Science of Learning, 3, 8. 10.1038/s41539-018-0024-y First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar