Skip to main content
Open AccessOriginal Article

Effects of Idle Time on Well-Being: An Experimental Study

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0932-4089/a000422

Abstract: Idle time is a time during which employees are unable to complete work tasks for reasons beyond their control, which negatively impacts their performance and well-being. However, it has not yet been examined whether the objective event or the subjective appraisal of this situation causes these effects. Drawing on affective events theory, we conducted an experiment (N = 338) in which we manipulated objective idle time and measured the effects on well-being, mediated by subjective idle time. We found that objective idle time positively predicted subjective idle time, which in turn was negatively related to affect but not to task satisfaction. We found indirect effects of objective idle time on affect through subjective idle time. Boredom proneness was positively and age was negatively associated with subjective idle time. The results are consistent with affective events theory and suggest that idle time is appraised as such and negatively influences affect.


Auswirkungen von Leerlaufzeiten auf das Wohlbefinden. Eine experimentelle Studie

Zusammenfassung: Leerlaufzeiten sind Zeiten, in denen Beschäftigte aus Gründen, die außerhalb ihrer Kontrolle liegen, ihre Arbeitsaufgaben nicht erledigen können, was sich negativ auf ihre Leistung und ihr Wohlbefinden auswirkt. Ob das objektive Ereignis oder die subjektive Bewertung dieser Situation diese Auswirkungen verursacht, wurde bisher nicht untersucht. Auf Grundlage der Theorie der affektiven Ereignisse haben wir ein Experiment (N = 338) durchgeführt, bei dem wir objektive Leerlaufzeit manipuliert und die Auswirkungen auf das Wohlbefinden gemessen haben, mediiert durch subjektive Leerlaufzeit. Wir fanden, dass objektive Leerlaufzeit die subjektive Leerlaufzeit positiv vorhersagte, die negativ mit Affekt, aber nicht mit Aufgabenzufriedenheit assoziiert war. Wir fanden indirekte Effekte der objektiven Leerlaufzeit auf den Affekt durch subjektive Leerlaufzeit. Die Neigung zur Langeweile war positiv und Alter negativ mit der subjektiven Leerlaufzeit assoziiert. Die Ergebnisse stimmen mit der Theorie der affektiven Ereignisse überein und legen nahe, dass Leerlaufzeit als solche bewertet wird und den Affekt negativ beeinflusst.

Literatur

  • Aghababaei, N., & Arji, A. (2014). Well-being and the HEXACO model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 139 – 142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.037 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Allman, M. J., Teki, S., Griffiths, T. D., & Meck, W. H. (2014). Properties of the internal clock: First- and second-order principles of subjective time. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 743 – 771. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115117 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2007). Empirical, theoretical, and practical advantages of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11 (2), 150 – 166. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868306294907 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2009). The HEXACO-60: A short measure of the major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91 (4), 340 – 345. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223890902935878 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baethge, A., & Rigotti, T. (2013). Interruptions to workflow: Their relationship with irritation and satisfaction with performance, and the mediating roles of time pressure and mental demands. Work & Stress, 27 (1), 43 – 63. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2013.761783 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Basch, J., & Fisher, C. D. (1998). Affective events – emotions matrix: A classification of work events and associated emotions. School of Business Discussion Papers 65. Retrieved from http://epublications.bond.edu.au/discussionpapers/65 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brissett, D., & Snow, R. P. (1993). Boredom: Where the future isn’t. Symbolic Interaction, 16 (3), 237 – 256. https://doi.org/10.1525/si.1993.16.3.237 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brodsky, A., & Amabile, T. M. (2018). The downside of downtime: The prevalence and work pacing consequences of idle time at work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 103 (5), 496 – 512. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000294 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97 (1), 19 – 35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cervone, D., & Pervin, L. A. (2019). Personality: Theory and research (14th ed.). Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cham, B. S., Andrei, D. M., Griffin, M. A., Grech, M., & Neal, A. (2021). Investigating the joint effects of overload and underload on chronic fatigue and well-being. Work & Stress, 35 (4), 344 – 357. https://doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2021.1888822 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Christandl, F., Mierke, K., & Peifer, C. (2018). Time flows: Manipulations of subjective time progression affect recalled flow and performance in a subsequent task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 74, 246 – 256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.09.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Colbert, A. E., Mount, M. K., Harter, J. K., Witt, L. A., & Barrick, M. R. (2004). Interactive effects of personality and perceptions of the work situation on workplace deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89 (4), 599 – 609. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.4.599 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cropanzano, R., & Dasborough, M. T. (2015). Dynamic models of well-being: Implications of affective events theory for expanding current views on personality and climate. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 24 (6), 844 – 847. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2015.1072245 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95 (3), 542 – 575. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and open questions in the science of subjective well-being. Collabra. Psychology, 4 (1), 1 – 78. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.115 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Donaldson, S. I., & Grant-Vallone, E. J. (2002). Understanding self-report bias in organizational behavior research. Journal of Business and Psychology, 17 (2), 245 – 260. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1019637632584 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Edwards, J. R., Caplan, R. D., & Harrison, R. V. (1998). Person-environment fit theory: Conceptual foundations, empirical evidence, and directions for future research. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Theories of organizational stress (pp. 28 – 67). Oxford. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Efrat-Treister, D., Daniels, M. A., & Robinson, S. L. (2020). Putting time in perspective: How and why construal level buffers the relationship between wait time and aggressive tendencies. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 41 (3), 294 – 309. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2433 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.–G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41 (4), 1149 – 1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1994). Action as the core of work psychology: A German approach. In H. C. TriandisM. D. DunnetteL. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 271 – 340). Consulting Psychologists. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Furnham, A. (1986). Response bias, social desirability and dissimulation. Personality and Individual Differences, 7 (3), 385 – 400. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(86)90014-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gana, K. (2018). Structural equation with lavaan. Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haarhaus, B. (2016). Entwicklung und Validierung eines Kurzfragebogens zur Erfassung von allgemeiner und facettenspezifischer Arbeitszufriedenheit [Development and validation of a short questionnaire for the assessment of general and facet-specific job satisfaction]. Diagnostica, 62 (2), 61 – 73. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000136 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Haehner, P., Rakhshani, A., Fassbender, I., Lucas, R. E., Donnellan, M. B., & Luhmann, M. (2022). Perception of major life events and personality trait change. European Journal of Personality, 089020702211079 https://doi.org/10.1177/08902070221107973 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hanfstingl, B., Arzenšek, A., Apschner, J., & Gölly, K. I. (2022). Assimilation and accommodation: A systematic review of the last two decades. European Psychologist, 27 (4), 320 – 337. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000463 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Haratsis, J. M., Creed, P. A., & Hood, M. (2015). Measuring assimilative and accommodative resources in young adults: Development and initial validation of suitable scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 61 – 66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30 (3), 576 – 598. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harrison, R. L. (2010). Introduction to Monte Carlo simulation. AIP Conference Proceedings, 1204, 17 – 21. https://doi.org/10.1063/1.3295638 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hayes, A. F., & Coutts, J. J. (2020). Use omega rather than Cronbach’s alpha for estimating reliability. But… Communication Methods and Measures, 14 (1), 1 – 24. https://doi.org/10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1), 1 – 55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hunter, J. A., Abraham, E. H., Hunter, A. G., Goldberg, L. C., & Eastwood, J. D. (2016). Personality and boredom proneness in the prediction of creativity and curiosity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 48 – 57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.08.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Judge, T. A., & Klinger, R. (2020). Job satisfaction. In V. I. SessaN. A. Bowling (Eds.), Essentials of job attitudes and other workplace psychological constructs (pp. 393 – 413). Routledge. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Junça‐Silva, A., Pombeira, C., & Caetano, A. (2021). Testing the affective events theory: The mediating role of affect and the moderating role of mindfulness. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 35 (4), 1075 – 1081. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3843 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kenny, D. A., Kaniskan, B., & McCoach, D. B. (2015). The performance of RMSEA in models with small degrees of freedom. Sociological Methods & Research, 44 (3), 486 – 507. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124114543236 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer Publishing. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412952576.n198 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lei, X., Kaplan, S. A., Dye, C. E., & Wong, C. M. (2019). On the subjective experience and correlates of downtime at work: A mixed-method examination. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 40 (3), 360 – 381. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.2336 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Li, Y., Ahlstrom, D., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2010). A multilevel model of affect and organizational commitment. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 27 (2), 193 – 213. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10490-010-9193-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luhmann, M., Fassbender, I., Alcock, M., & Haehner, P. (2021). A dimensional taxonomy of perceived characteristics of major life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121 (3), 633 – 668. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000291 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lyubykh, Z., Gulseren, D., Premji, Z., Wingate, T. G., Deng, C., Bélanger, L. J., & Turner, N. (2022). Role of work breaks in well-being and performance: A systematic review and future research agenda. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 27 (5), 470 – 487. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000337 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miscenko, D., & Day, D. V. (2016). Identity and identification at work. Organizational Psychology Review, 6 (3), 215 – 247. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041386615584009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mojzisch, A., & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2007). Being fed up: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to mental satiation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1118, 186 – 205. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1412.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morgeson, F. P., Mitchell, T. R., & Liu, D. (2015). Event system theory: An event-oriented approach to the organizational sciences. Academy of Management Review, 40 (4), 515 – 537. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2012.0099 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mulder, J. D., & Hamaker, E. L. (2020). Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 1 – 11. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705511.2020.1784738 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nah, F. F.–H. (2004). A study on tolerable waiting time: How long are Web users willing to wait? Behaviour & Information Technology, 23 (3), 153 – 163. https://doi.org/10.1080/01449290410001669914 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2009). Flow theory and research. In C. R. SnyderS. J. Lopez (Eds.), Oxford library of psychology. Oxford handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 195 – 206). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ohly, S., Sonnentag, S., Niessen, C., & Zapf, D. (2010). Diary studies in organizational research. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9 (2), 79 – 93. https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000009 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1989). The stress appraisal measure (SAM). Stress Medicine, 6, 227 – 236. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peifer, C., Syrek, C., Ostwald, V., Schuh, E., & Antoni, C. H. (2020). Thieves of flow: How unfinished tasks at work are related to flow experience and well-being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 21 (5), 1641 – 1660. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-019-00149-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peifer, C., & Tan, J. (2021). The psychophysiology of flow experience. In C. PeiferS. Engeser (Eds.), Advances in flow research (pp. 191 – 230). Springer International. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-53468-4_8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Puranik, H., Koopman, J., & Vough, H. C. (2020). Pardon the interruption: An integrative review and future research agenda for research on work interruptions. Journal of Management, 46 (6), 806 – 842. https://doi.org/10.1177/0149206319887428 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rauthmann, J. F., & Sherman, R. A. (2020). The situation of situation research: Knowns and unknowns. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29 (5), 473 – 480. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420925546 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48 (2), 1 – 36. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosseel, Y. (2021). Lavaan (Version 0.6.9) [Computer software]. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ryffel, F. A. (2017). Alpha and beta error (accumulation). In J. MatthesC. S. DavisR. F. Potter (Eds.), The international encyclopedia of communication research methods (pp. 1 – 5). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118901731.iecrm0002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sackett, P. R., & Lievens, F. (2008). Personnel selection. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 419 – 450. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093716 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schaffer, R. H. (1953). Job satisfaction as related to need satisfaction in work. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 67 (14), 1 – 29. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schubert, K., Zeschke, M., & Zacher, H. (2023). What to do when there is nothing to do? Toward a better understanding of idle time at work. Current Psychology (42), 11871 – 11890. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02445-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schweisfurth, T. G., & Greul, A. (2023). Unexpected interruptions, idle time, and creativity: Evidence from a natural experiment. Organization Science. Advance online publication https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2023.1660 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Struk, A. A., Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., & Danckert, J. (2017). A short boredom proneness scale. Assessment, 24 (3), 346 – 359. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115609996 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Syrek, C. J., Weigelt, O., Peifer, C., & Antoni, C. H. (2017). Zeigarnik’s sleepless nights: How unfinished tasks at the end of the week impair employee sleep on the weekend through rumination. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 22 (2), 225 – 238. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000031 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taber, T. D., & Alliger, G. M. (1995). A task-level assessment of job satisfaction. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 16 (2), 101 – 121. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.4030160202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tam, K. Y. Y., van Tilburg, W. A. P., & Chan, C. S. (2021). What is boredom proneness? A comparison of three characterizations. Journal of Personality, 89 (4), 831 – 846. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12618 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wearden, J. (2016). Timing and ageing. In J. Wearden (Ed.), The psychology of time perception (pp. 167 – 181). Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-40883-9_8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, cause and consequences of affective experiences at work. In B. M. StawL. L. Cummings (Ed.), Research in organizational behavior: An annual series of analytical essays and critical reviews (Vol. 18, pp. 1 – 74). JAI. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weiss, H. M., & Nowicki, C. E. (1981). Social influences on task satisfaction: Model competence and observer field dependence. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 27 (3), 345 – 366. https://doi.org/10.1016/0030-5073(81)90028-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wittmann, M., & Lehnhoff, S. (2005). Age effects in perception of time. Psychological Reports, 97 (3), 921 – 935. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.97.3.921-935 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zeschke, M., & Zacher, H. (2023). Is it bad because it is boring? Effects of idle time on employee outcomes. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 1 – 15. https://doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2023.2261661 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar