Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024//0044-3514.31.2.87

Zusammenfassung: In dieser Arbeit werden eine deutschsprachige Skala zur Erfassung intuitiver und rationaler Verarbeitung (Übersetzung des Rational-Experiential Inventory [REI]; Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj & Heier, 1996) sowie neue Skalen zur Verwendung und Bewertung verschiedener Persuasionsheuristiken vorgestellt. Das deutsche REI weist gute Itemcharakteristika und hohe Reliabilität auf und repliziert die zweidimensionale Struktur der Originalskala mit den Subskalen «faith in intuition» und «need for cognition». Beide Dimensionen sind unabhängig von sozialer Erwünschtheit, und Korrelationen mit verschiedenen Persönlichkeitsmerkmalen belegen ihre Konstruktvalidität. Weitere korrelative Ergebnisse zeigen Unterschiede in der selbstberichteten Nutzung verschiedener Persuasionsheuristiken in Abhängigkeit von «faith in intuition» und «need for cognition», die darauf hinweisen, dass zwischen Regel basierten und assoziativen heuristischen Prozessen unterschieden werden kann. Zudem zeigen unsere Befunde auf korrelativer Basis, dass eine konzeptuelle Trennung von heuristischer und intuitiver Verarbeitung geboten scheint.


Summary:In this paper we present a German scale assessing intuitive and rational processing (a translation of the Rational-Experiential Inventory [REI]; Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj & Heier, 1996) as well as new scales assessing the use and evaluation of various persuasion heuristics. The German REI is shown to have good item chracteristics and high reliability, and to replicate the two-dimensional structure of the original scale with its subscales faith in intuition and need for cognition. Both dimensions are independent of social desirability, and correlations with various personality traits speak to their construct validity. Further results indicate differences in the self-reported use of different persuasion heuristics contingent on the need for cognition and faith in intuition that hint at a differentiation of associative and rule based heuristic processes. In addition, the correlational patterns suggest a conceptual distinction between heuristic and intuitive processing.

Literatur

  • Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment . Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology , 42, 155– 162 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bless, H. , Wänke, M. , Bohner, G. , Fellhauer, R. F. , Schwarz, N. (1994). Need for Cognition: Eine Skala zur Erfassung von Engagement und Freude bei Denkaufgaben. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie , 25, 147– 154 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bohner, G. , Moskowitz, G. , Chaiken, S. (1995). The interplay of heuristic and systematic processing of social information. European Review of Social Psychology , 6, 33– 68 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression formation. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Eds.), Advances in social cognition (Bd. 1, S. 1-36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T. , Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 42, 116– 131 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T. , Petty, R. E. , Feinstein, J. A. , Jarvis, W. B. G. (1996). Dispositional differences in cognitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition. Psychological Bulletin , 119, 197– 253 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chaiken, S. , Giner-Sorolla, R. , Chen, S. (1996). Beyond accuracy: Defense and impression motives in heuristic and systematic processing. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.). The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 553-578). New York: Guilford . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chaiken, S. , Liberman, A. , Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and systematic information processing within and beyond the persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.). Unintended thought (pp. 212-252). New York: Guilford . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chaiken, S. , Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 66, 460– 473 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, S. , Chaiken, S. (1999). The heuristic-systematic model in its broader context. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual-process theories in social psychology. (pp. 73-96). New York: Guilford . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Crowne, D. P. , Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology , 24, 349– 354 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeBono, K. G. , Harnish, R. J. (1988). Source expertise, source attractiveness, and the processing of persuasive information: A functional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 55, 541– 546 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dornic, S. , Ekehammer, B. , Laaksonen, T. (1991). Tolerance for mental effort: Self-ratings related to perception, performance, and personality. Personality and Individual Differences , 12, 313– 319 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Edwards, J. A. , Weary, G. , Reich, D. A. (1998). Causal uncertainty: Factor structure and relation to the big five personality factors. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 24, 451– 462 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenberg, N. , Martin, L. , Fabes, R. A. (1996). Gender development and gender effects. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 358-396). New York: Macmillan . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory. In L. Pervin (Hrsg.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 165-192). New York: Guilford Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S. , Lipson, A. , Holstein, C. , Huh, E. (1992). Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 62, 328– 339 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S. , Pacini, R. , Denes-Raj, V. , Heier., H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 71, 390– 405 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Erb, H.-P. , Bohner, G. , Schmälzle, K. , Rank, S. (1998). Beyond conflict and discrepancy: Cognitive bias in minority and majority influence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 24, 620– 633 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology , 23, 75– 109 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. , Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. In M. P. Zanna (Hrsg.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1-74). New York: Academic Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 189-211). New York: Guilford . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gough, H. G. (1969). The CPI manual . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , Bnaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review , 102, 4– 27 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. G. , Banaji, M. R. , Rudman, L. A. , Farnham, S. D. , Nosek, B. A. , Rosier, M. (2000). Prologue to a unified theory of attitudes, stereotypes, and self-concept. In J. P. Forgas (Eds.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition and behavior (pp. 308-330). New York: Cambridge University Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hall, W. B. , MacKinnon, D. W. (1969). Personality inventory correlates of creativity among architects. Journal of Applied Psychology , 53, 322– 326 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harris, M. L. , Harris, C. W. (1971). A factor analytic interpretation strategy. Educational and Psychological Measurement , 31, 589– 606 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahneman, D. , Slovic, P. , Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases . New York: Cambridge University Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W. , Thompson, E. P. (1999). Persuasion by a single route: A view from the unimodel. Psychological Inquiry , 10, 83– 109 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lück, H. E. , Timaeus, E. (1969). Skalen zur Messung Manifester Angst (MAS) und sozialer Erwünschtheit (SDS-E und SDS-CM). Diagnostica , 15, 134– 141 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Maccoby, E. E. , Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences . Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McCrae, R. R. , Costa, P. T. (1987). Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 52, 81– 90 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Norman, W. T. (1963). Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: Replicated factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology , 66, 574– 583 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ostendorf, F. (1990). Sprache und Persönlichkeitsstruktur: Zur Validität des Fünf-Faktoren-Modells der Persönlichkeit . Regensburg: Roderer . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pallak, S. R. , Murroni, E. , Koch, J. (1983). Communicator attractiveness and expertise, emotional versus rational appeals, and persuasion: A heuristic versus systematic processing interpretation. Social Cognition , 2, 122– 141 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology , 19, 124– 203 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Cacioppo, J. T. , Schumann, D. (1983). Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research , 10, 134– 148 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Harkins, S. G. , Williams, K. D. (1980). The effects of group diffusion of cognitive effort on attitudes: An information processing view. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 38, 81– 92 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Wegener, D. T. (1999). The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 41-72). New York: Guilford . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosch, E. (1983). Prototype classification and logical classification: The two systems. In E. Scholnick (Eds.), New trends in conceptual representation: Challenges to Piaget's theory (pp. 73-86). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image . Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schneider-Düker, M. , Kohler, A. (1988). Die Erfassung von Geschlechtsrollen -Ergebnisse zur deutschen Neukonstruktion des Bem-Sex-Role-Inventory. Diagnostica , 34, 256– 272 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schulz, R. , Ewen, R. (1993). Adult development and aging (2. Aufl.). New York: Macmillan . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin , 119, 3– 22 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, E. R. , DeCoster, J. (1997). Heuristic-systematic and other dual process models in social and cognitive psychology: An integration and connectionist interpretation (Unpublished manuscript). West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Spielberger, C. D. , Gorsuch, R. L. , Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory . Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tversky, A. , Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning. The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review , 90, 293– 315 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wood, W. , Lundgren, S. , Ouellette, J. A. , Busceme, S. , Blackstone, T. (1994). Minority influence: A meta-analytic review of social influence processes. Psychological Bulletin , 115, 323– 345 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar