Skip to main content
Original Article

Where do pre-service teachers' educational psychological misconceptions come from?

The roles of anecdotal versus scientific evidence

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1010-0652/a000299

Abstract. Previous research has found a high prevalence of some (educational) psychological misconceptions (i.e., incorrect but often popular assumptions that contradict results from psychological research) among (pre-service) teachers. However, the number of topics that have been investigated is limited. Additionally, knowing the sources of misconceptions might be helpful for rebutting them. Furthermore, anecdotal evidence has been found to be important for informing (pre-service) teachers' practice, but personal experiences also are among the main sources of misconceptions. Therefore, we hypothesized that pre-service teachers would predominantly view sources of anecdotal evidence as the origin of their educational psychological beliefs in general and the main source of their misconceptions in particular. In an online survey (with correlational and quasi-experimental elements) of N = 836 pre-service teachers, we found that educational psychological misconceptions were less prevalent than expected but that pre-service teachers indeed mainly based their beliefs on sources of anecdotal evidence (personal experiences and narratives from other people) and that these nonscientific sources turned out to be the main sources of their misconceptions (comparison with scientific sources: d = 0.19 and d = 0.23). Furthermore, referring more to sources of anecdotal than scientific evidence (research and lectures) was associated with undesirable aspects, that is, more misconceptions (d = 0.21) and less reduction of misconception endorsement through empirical refutation-style information (d = 0.30) but not with a lower judgment of the view that it is possible to examine educational psychological topics scientifically. In sum, our results indicate that basing one's beliefs more on sources of anecdotal than scientific evidence is associated with outcomes that stand in contrast to evidence-based education. Future research should investigate why pre-service teachers concentrate on sources of anecdotal evidence, how to make sources of scientific evidence more tempting, and whether counteracting misconceptions by showing the downside of nonscientific sources is effective.


Woher kommen pädagogisch-psychologische Fehlvorstellungen von Lehramtsstudierenden? Die Rolle anekdotischer und wissenschaftlicher Evidenzen

Zusammenfassung. Bisherige Forschung weist auf eine hohe Prävalenz einiger (pädagogisch-)psychologischer Fehlvorstellungen (d.h. einem Glauben an falsche und verbreitete Annahmen, die wissenschaftlichen Erkenntnissen der Psychologie widersprechen) unter (angehenden) Lehrkräften hin. Allerdings ist die Anzahl der bisher untersuchten Themen begrenzt. Darüber hinaus kann es für die Bekämpfung von Fehlvorstellungen hilfreich sein, deren Quellen zu kennen. Da anekdotisches Wissen für professionelle Entscheidungen von (angehenden) Lehrkräften eine große Rolle spielt und persönliche Erfahrungen zu den am meisten genannten Quellen von Fehlvorstellungen gehören, nahmen wir an, dass angehende Lehrkräfte grundsätzlich vor allem anekdotische Quellen als Basis ihrer pädagogisch-psychologischen Überzeugungen nennen würden und sich diese auch als Hauptquelle ihrer pädagogisch-psychologischen Fehlvorstellungen im Speziellen erweisen würden. In einer Online-Befragung (mit sowohl korrelativen als auch quasi-experimentellen Elementen) an N = 836 Lehramtsstudierenden fanden wir einerseits, dass pädagogisch-psychologische Fehlvorstellungen weniger verbreitet waren als angenommen. Andererseits zeigte sich, dass die Lehramtsstudierenden ihre Überzeugungen tatsächlich hauptsächlich auf anekdotische Quellen (eigene oder stellvertretende Erfahrungen) zurückführten und dass sich diese auch als die Hauptquelle ihrer Fehlvorstellungen herausstellten (Vergleich mit wissenschaftlichen Quellen: d = 0.19 bzw. d = 0.23). Zusätzlich fanden wir, dass die vermehrte Nennung von anekdotischen Quellen im Vergleich zu wissenschaftlichen Quellen (Publikationen und Lehrveranstaltungen) als Ursprung von Überzeugungen mit unerwünschten Aspekten einherging, nämlich mit mehr Fehlvorstellungen (d = 0.21) und mit einer geringeren Reduktion von Fehlvorstellungen durch empirische widerlegende Texte (d = 0.30) – jedoch nicht mit einem geringeren Glauben an die wissenschaftliche Untersuchbarkeit pädagogisch-psychologischer Themen. Unsere Ergebnisse deuten insgesamt darauf hin, dass ein vermehrtes Verlassen auf anekdotische im Vergleich zu wissenschaftlichen Quellen mit Aspekten assoziiert ist, die in Kontrast zu evidenzbasierter Bildung stehen. Zukünftige Forschung sollte untersuchen, weshalb sich angehende Lehrkräfte auf eigene oder stellvertretende Erfahrungen konzentrieren, wie man wissenschaftliche Quellen ansprechender gestalten kann und, ob eine Bekämpfung von Fehlvorstellungen durch das Aufzeigen von Nachteilen nicht-wissenschaftlicher Quellen effektiv ist.

References

  • Allen, J. M. (2009). Valuing practice over theory: How beginning teachers re-orient their practice in the transition from the university to the workplace. Teaching and Teacher Education , 25 , 647–654. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2008.11.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Appel, M. & Richter, T. (2007). Persuasive effects of fictional narratives increase over time. Media Psychology , 10 , 113–134. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213260701301194 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bensley, D. A. & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2015). What is a psychological misconception? Moving toward an empirical answer. Teaching of Psychology , 42 , 282–292. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628315603059 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bensley, D. A. & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2017). Psychological misconceptions: Recent scientific advances and unresolved issues. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 26 , 377–382. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417699026 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bråten, I. & Ferguson, L. E. (2015). Beliefs about sources of knowledge predict motivation for learning in teacher education. Teaching and Teacher Education , 50 , 13–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2015.04.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buehl, M. M. & Fives, H. (2009). Exploring teachers' beliefs about teaching knowledge: Where does it come from? Does it change? Journal of Experimental Education , 77 , 367–407. https://doi.org/10.3200/JEXE.77.4.367-408 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cain, T. (2016). Research utilisation and the struggle for the teacher's soul: A narrative review. European Journal of Teacher Education , 39 , 616–629. https://doi.org/10.1080/02619768.2016.1252912 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chew, S. L. (2006). Seldom in doubt but often wrong: Addressing tenacious student misconceptions. In D. S. Dunn S. L. Chew (Eds.), Best practices for teaching introduction to psychology (pp.211–224). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences ( 2nd ed. ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Commission of the European Communities (2007). Communication from the commission to the council and the European parliament. Improving the quality of teacher education . Retrieved from https://eur-lex.europa.eu/legal-content/EN/TXT/?uri=celex%3A52007DC0392 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dagenais, C. , Lysenko, L. , Abrami, P. C. , Bernard, R. M. , Ramde, J. & Janosz, M. (2012). Use of research-based information by school practitioners and determinants of use: A review of empirical research. Evidence & Policy , 8 , 285–309. https://doi.org/10.1332/174426412X654031 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Bruyckere, P. , Kirschner, P. A. & Hulshof, C. D. (2015). Urban myths about learning and education . London: Elsevier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dekker, S. , Lee, N. C. , Howard-Jones, P. & Jolles, J. (2012). Neuromyths in education: Prevalence and predictors of misconceptions among teachers. Frontiers in Psychology , 3 (429), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00429 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Doran, P. T. & Zimmerman, M. K. (2009). Examining the scientific consensus on climate change. Eos, Transactions American Geophysical Union , 90 , 22–23. https://doi.org/10.1029/2009EO030002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunn, D. S. , Saville, B. K. , Baker, S. C. & Marek, P. (2013). Evidence-based teaching: Tools and techniques that promote learning in the psychology classroom. Australian Journal of Psychology , 65 , 5–13. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dündar, S. & Gündüz, N. (2016). Misconceptions regarding the brain: The neuromyths of pre-service teachers. Mind, Brain, and Education , 10 , 212–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12119 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Funder, D. C. & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science , 2 , 156–168. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919847202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Furnham, A. & Hughes, D. J. (2014). Myths and misconceptions in popular psychology: Comparing psychology students and the general public. Teaching of Psychology , 41 , 256–261. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628314537984 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gardner, R. M. & Dalsing, S. (1986). Misconceptions about psychology among college students. Teaching of Psychology , 13 , 32–34. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328023top1301_9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gaze, C. M. (2014). Popular psychological myths: A comparison of students' beliefs across the psychology major. Journal of the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning , 14 , 46–60. https://doi.org/10.14434/josotl.v14i2.3931 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ghanizadeh, A. , Bahredar, M. J. & Moeini, S. R. (2006). Knowledge and attitudes towards attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among elementary school teachers. Patient Education and Counseling , 63 , 84–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2005.09.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn't so: The fallibility of human reason in everyday life . New York: Free Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gitlin, A. , Barlow, L. , Burbank, M. D. , Kauchak, D. & Stevens, T. (1999). Pre-service teachers' thinking on research: Implications for inquiry oriented teacher education. Teaching and Teacher Education , 15 , 753–769. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0742-051X(99)00015-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hamza, K. M. & Wickman, P.-O. (2008). Describing and analyzing learning in action: An empirical study of the importance of misconceptions in learning science. Science Education , 92 , 141–164. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.20233 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hargreaves, D. H. (2000). Teaching as a research-based profession: Possibilities and prospects. In B. Moon J. Butcher E. Bird (Eds.), Leading professional development in education (pp.200–210). London: RoutledgeFalmer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hermida, M. J. , Segretin, M. S. , Soni García, A. & Lipina, S. J. (2016). Conceptions and misconceptions about neuroscience in preschool teachers: A study from Argentina. Educational Research , 58 , 457–472. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131881.2016.1238585 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Higbee, K. L. & Clay, S. L. (1998). College students' beliefs in the ten-percent myth. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied , 132 , 469–476. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223989809599280 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Howard-Jones, P. , Franey, L. , Mashmoushi, R. & Liao, Y.-C. (2009). The neuroscience literacy of trainee teachers . Paper presented at British Educational Research Association Annual Conference, Manchester. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, S. , Lyddy, F. , Kaplan, R. , Nichols, A. L. , Miller, H. , Saad, C. G. et al. (2015). Highly prevalent but not always persistent: Undergraduate and graduate student's misconceptions about psychology. Teaching of Psychology , 42 , 34–42. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628314562677 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hughes, S. , Lyddy, F. & Lambe, S. (2013). Misconceptions about psychological science: A review. Psychology Learning & Teaching , 12 , 20–31. https://doi.org/10.2304/plat.2013.12.1.20 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ingram, D. , Louis, K. & Schroeder, R. G. (2004). Accountability policies and teacher decision making: Barriers to the use of data to improve practice. Teachers College Record , 106 , 1258–1287. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9620.2004.00379.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kendeou, P. & van den Broek, P. (2005). The effects of readers' misconceptions on comprehension of scientific text. Journal of Educational Psychology , 97 , 235–245. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.235 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • KMK [Kultusministerkonferenz] (2004). Standards für die Lehrerbildung: Bildungswissenschaften. Beschluss der Kultusministerkonferenz. [ Standards for teacher education. Decision of the Conference of the Ministers of Education and Cultural Affairs. ] Bonn: KMK. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kowalski, P. & Taylor, A. (2009). The effect of refuting misconceptions in the introductory psychology class. Teaching of Psychology , 36 , 153–159. https://doi.org/10.1080/00986280902959986 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Landau, J. D. & Bavaria, A. J. (2003). Does deliberate source monitoring reduce students' misconceptions about psychology? Teaching of Psychology , 30 , 311–314. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lassonde, K. A. , Kolquist, M. & Vergin, M. (2017). Revising psychology misconceptions by integrating a refutation-style text framework into poster presentations. Teaching of Psychology , 44 , 255–262. https://doi.org/10.1177/0098628317712754 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lilienfeld, S. O. (2005). Challenging mind myths in introductory psychology courses. Psychology Teacher Network , 15 (3), 1–6. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lilienfeld, S. O. , Ammirati, R. & David, M. (2012). Distinguishing science from pseudoscience in school psychology: Science and scientific thinking as safeguards against human error. Journal of School Psychology , 50 (1), 7–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2011.09.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lilienfeld, S. O. , Lynn, S. , Ruscio, J. & Beyerstein, B. (2010). 50 great myths of popular psychology . Oxford, England: Wiley-Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Marsh, E. J. , Cantor, A. D. & Brashier, N. M. (2016). Believing that humans swallow spiders in their sleep: False beliefs as side effects of the processes that support accurate knowledge. Psychology of Learning and Motivation , 64 , 93–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2015.09.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mason, L. , Baldi, R. , Di Ronco, S. , Scrimin, S. , Danielson, R. W. & Sinatra, G. M. (2017). Textual and graphical refutations: Effects on conceptual change learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology , 49 , 275–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.03.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCutcheon, L. E. (1991). A new test of misconceptions about psychology. Psychological Reports , 68 , 647–653. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1991.68.2.647 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Menz, C. , Spinath, B. & Seifried, E. (2020). Misconceptions die hard: Prevalence and reduction of wrong beliefs in topics from educational psychology among preservice teachers. European Journal of Psychology of Education . Advanced online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-020-00474-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Munro, G. D. (2010). The scientific impotence excuse: Discounting belief-threatening scientific abstracts. Journal of Applied Social Psychology , 40 , 579–600. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00588.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pasquinelli, E. (2012). Neuromyths: Why do they exist and persist? Mind, Brain, and Education , 6 , 89–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2012.01141.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pieschl, S. , Archer, J. & Budd, J. (2019). Raising student teachers' awareness of misconception regarding educational psychology . Poster presented at the 2019 Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (AERA), Toronto, Canada. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Prinz, A. , Golke, S. & Wittwer, J. (2018). The double curse of misconceptions: Misconceptions impair not only text comprehension but also metacomprehension in the domain of statistics. Instructional Science , 46 , 723–765. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11251-018-9452-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prinz, A. , Golke, S. & Wittwer, J. (2019). Refutation texts compensate for detrimental effects of misconceptions on comprehension and metacomprehension accuracy and support transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology , 111 , 957–981. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000329 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richter, T. , Schroeder, S. & Wöhrmann, B. (2009). You don't have to believe everything you read: Background knowledge permits fast and efficient validation of information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 96 , 538–558. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014038 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richter, T. & Singer, M. (2017). Discourse updating: Acquiring and revising knowledge through discourse. In M. F. Schober D. N. Rapp M. A. Britt (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of discourse processes (pp.167–190). New York: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315687384-11 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schildkamp, K. & Kuiper, W. (2010). Data-informed curriculum reform: Which data, what purposes, and promoting and hindering factors. Teaching and Teacher Education , 26 , 482–496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2009.06.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slavin, R. E. (2002). Evidence-based education policies: Transforming educational practice and research. Educational Researcher , 31 , 15–21. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X031007015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Slavin, R. E. (2008). Perspectives on evidence-based research in education – what works? Issues in synthesizing educational program evaluations. Educational Researcher , 37 , 5–14. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X08314117 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, A. K. & Kowalski, P. (2004). Naive psychological science: The prevalence, strength, and sources of misconceptions. Psychological Record , 54 , 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395459 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, A. K. & Kowalski, P. (2014). Student misconceptions: Where do they come from and what can we do? In V. A. Benassi C. E. Overson C. M. Hakala (Eds.), Applying science of learning in education: Infusing psychological science into the curriculum (pp.259–273). American Psychological Association (Division 2). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tippett, C. D. (2010). Refutational text in science education: A review of two decades of research. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education , 8 , 951–970. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10763-010-9203-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vanderlinde, R. & van Braak, J. (2010). The gap between educational research and practice: Views of teachers, school leaders, intermediaries and researchers. British Educational Research Journal , 36 , 299–316. https://doi.org/10.1080/01411920902919257 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Schaik, P. , Volman, M. , Admiraal, W. & Schenke, W. (2018). Barriers and conditions for teachers' utilisation of academic knowledge. International Journal of Educational Research , 90 , 50–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijer.2018.05.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vaughan, E. D. (1977). Misconceptions about psychology among introductory psychology students. Teaching of Psychology , 4 , 138–141. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15328023top0403_9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar