Skip to main content
Original Article

Face Memory in Idiopathic Parkinson’s Disease Moderated by Sex and Encoding Duration

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1016-264X/a000148

We examined face memory deficits in patients with Idiopathic Parkinson’s disease (IPD) with specific regard to the moderating role of sex and the different memory processes involved. We tested short- and long-term face recognition memory in 18 nonclinical participants and 18 IPD-patients matched for sex, education and age. We varied the duration of item presentation (1, 5, 10s), the time of testing (immediately, 1hr, 24hrs) and the possibility to re-encode items. In accordance with earlier studies, we report face memory deficits in IPD. Moreover, our findings indicate that sex and encoding conditions may be important moderator variables. In contrast to healthy individuals, IPD-patients cannot gain from increasing duration of presentation. Furthermore, our results suggest that IPD leads to face memory deficits in women, only.


Gesichtergedächtnis bei idiopathischem Parkinson-Syndrom, moderiert durch das Geschlecht und die Enkodierdauer

Die Studie untersuchte Gesichtergedächtnisdefizite bei Patienten mit idiopathischem Parkinson-Syndrom (IPS). Unter der speziellen Berücksichtigung der moderierenden Funktion des Geschlechts und der verschiedenen involvierten Gedächtnisprozesse, wurden Kurz- und Langzeit-Gesichtergedächtnis bei 18 nicht-klinischen Probanden und 18 IPS-Patienten getestet. Beide Gruppen waren vergleichbar hinsichtlich des Geschlechts, der Bildung und des Alters. Wir variierten die Dauer der Item-Präsentation (1, 5, 10 s), den Abrufzeitpunkt (unmittelbar, 1 Std, 24 Std), sowie die Möglichkeit, die Items zu re-enkodieren. In Übereinstimmung mit früheren Studien können Gesichtergedächtnisdefizite bei Patienten mit IPS berichtet werden. Überdies deuten die Ergebnisse darauf hin, dass Geschlecht und Enkodierbedingung wichtige Moderatorvariablen sind. Im Unterschied zu gesunden Probanden, können Patienten mit IPS nicht von einer erhöhten Enkodierdauer profitieren. Darüber hinaus weisen die Ergebnisse darauf hin, dass IPS lediglich bei betroffenen Frauen zu Gesichtergedächtnisdefiziten führt.

References

  • Aalto, S., Brück, A., Laine, M., Nårgen, K. & Rinne, J. O. (2005). Frontal and temporal dopamine release during working memory and attention tasks in healthy humans: A positron emission tomography study using the high-affinity dopamine D2 receptor ligand [11C]FLB 457. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2471 – 2477. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aarsland, D., Zaccai, J. & Brayne, C. (2005). A systematic review of prevalence studies of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 20, 1255 – 1263. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aarts, E., van Holstein, M. & Cools, R. (2011). Striatal dopamine and the interface between motivation and cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, article 163. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ackenheil, M., Stotz-Ingenlath, G., Dietz-Bauer, R. & Vossen, A. (1999). M.I.N.I. Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview, German Version 5.0.0 DSM IV. München: Psychiatrische Universitätsklinik München. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Andreano, J. M. & Cahill, L. (2009). Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learning & Memory, 16, 248 – 266. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aster, M. von, Neubauer, A. & Horn, R. (2006). Wechsler Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (WIE). Deutschsprachige Bearbeitung und Adaptation des WAIS-III von David Wechsler. Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Aschenbrenner, S., Tucher, O. & Lange, K. W. (2000). Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test (Handbuch mit Protokollbögen). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bengner, T., Malina, T., Lindenau, M., Voges, B., Goebell, E. & Stodieck, S. (2006a). Face memory in MRI-negative and MRI‐positive temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 47, 1904 – 1914. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bengner, T., Fortmeier, C., Malina, T., Lindenau, M., Voges, B., Goebell, E. et al. (2006b). Sex differences in face recognition memory in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, patients with generalized epilepsy, and healthy controls. Epilepsy & Behavior, 9, 593 – 600. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benton A., Sivan, A. B., Hamsher, K., Verney, N. R. & Spreen, O. (1983). Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bortz, J. & Schuster, C. (2010). Statistik für Human- und Sozialwissenschaftler (7. Aufl.). Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Braak, H. & Braak, E. (2000). Pathoanatomy of Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, 247, 3 – 10. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brønnick, K., Alves, G., Aarsland, D., Tysnes, O. B. & Larsen, J. P. (2011). Verbal memory in drug-naive, newly diagnosed Parkinson’s disease. The retrieval deficit hypothesis revisited. Neuropsychology, 25, 114 – 124. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buckner, R. L., Kelley, W. M. & Petersen, S. E. (1999). Frontal cortex contributes to human memory formation. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 311 – 314. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burton, J. E., McKeith, G. I., Burn, J. D., Williams, D. E. & O’Brian, T. J. (2004). Cerebral atrophy in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia: A comparison with Alzheimer’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies and controls. Brain, 127, 791 – 800. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bussey, T. J. & Saksida, L. M. (2005). Object memory and perception in the medial temporal lobe: an alternative approach. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 730 – 737. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Calabrese, P., Kalbe, E. & Kessler, J. (2006). Parkinson Neuropsychometric Dementia Assessment (PANDA). Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A. & Ahluwalia, J. (2000). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 113 – 118. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cousins, R., Hanley, R., Davies, A. D. M., Turnbull, C. J. & Playfer, J. R. (2000). Understanding memory for faces in Parkinson’s disease: The role of configural processing. Neuropsychologia, 38, 837 – 847. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dewick, H. C., Hanley, J. R., Davies, A. D. M., Playfer, J. R. & Turnbull, C. J. (1991). Perception and memory for faces in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 29, 785 – 802. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dubois, B. & Pillon, B. (1997). Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurology, 244, 2 – 8. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duchaine, B. C. & Waidenfeld, A. (2003). An evaluation of two commonly used tests of unfamiliar face recognition. Neuropsychologia, 41, 713 – 720. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Emre, M. (2003). Dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. The Lancet Neurology, 2, 229 – 237. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Erixon-Lindroth, N., Farde, L., Wahlin, T. R., Sovago, J., Halldin, C. & Bäckman, L. (2005). The role of the striatal dopamine transporter in cognitive aging. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 138, 1 – 12. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fletcher, P. C., & Henson, R. N. A. (2001). Frontal lobes and human memory. Insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain, 124, 849 – 881. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frey, S. & Petrides, M. (2003). Greater orbitofrontal activity predicts better memory for faces. European Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 2755 – 2758. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gelb, D. J., Oliver, E. & Gilman, S. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for Parkinson disease. Archives of Neurology, 56, 33 – 39. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Golby, J. A., Poldrack, A. R., Brewer, B. J., Spencer, D., Desmond, E. J., Aron, P. A. et al. (2001). Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain, 124, 1841 – 1854. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grahn, A. J., Parkinson, A. J. & Owen, M. A. (2009). The role of the basal ganglia in learning and memory: Neuropsychological studies. Behavioral Brain Research, 199, 53 – 60. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, J., McDonald, W. M., Vitek, J. L., Evatt, M., Freeman, A. & Haber, M. (2002). Cognitive impairments in advanced PD without dementia. Neurology, 59, 1320 – 1324. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Härting, C., Markowitsch, H. J., Neufeld, H., Calabrese, P., Deisinger, K. & Kessler, J. (2000). Die Wechsler-Memory-Scale-revised. Deutschsprachige Adaptation. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Haeske-Dewick, H. C. (1996). Are perception and memory for faces influenced by a specifc age at onset factor in Parkinson’s disease? Neuropsychologia, 34, 315 – 320. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hautzinger, M., Bailer, M., Worall, H. & Keller, F. (1995). Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI); Testhandbuch (2. Aufl.). Bern: Hans Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Heinz, A. (1999). Anhedonia – a general nosology surmounting correlate of a dysfunctional dopaminergic reward system? Nervenarzt, 70, 391 – 398. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herlitz, A. & Lovén, J. (2009). Sex differences in cognitive functions. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 41, 1081 – 1090. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herlitz, A. & Rehnman, J. (2008). Sex differences in episodic memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 52 – 56. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herlitz, A. & Yonker, J. E. (2002). Sex differences in episodic memory: The influence of intelligence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 107 – 114. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herrmann-Lingen, C., Buss, U. & Snaith, R. P. (1995). Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS-D) – Deutsche Version. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hoehn, M. M. & Yahr, M. D. (1967). Parkinsonism: Onset, progression and mortality. Neurology, 17, 427 – 42. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jellinger, K. A. (1999). Post mortem studies in Parkinson’s disease – is it possible to detect brain areas for specific symptoms? Journal of Neural Transmission Supplementum, 56, 1 – 29. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jellinger, K. A. (2008). Dopamin im Gehirn. Teil 2: Seine Funktion und Bedeutung für neuropsychiatrische Erkrankungen. Psychopraxis, 1, Wien: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kaasinen, V., Någren, K., Hietala, J., Oikonen, V., Vilkman, H., Farde, L. et al. (2000). Extrastriatal dopamine D2 and D3 receptors in early and advanced Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 54, 1482 – 1487. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaasinen, V. & Rinne, J. O. (2002). Functional imaging studies of dopamine system and cognition in normal aging and Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 785 – 793. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Katzen, H. L., Levin, B. E. & Weiner, W. (2006). Side and type of motor symptom influence cognition in Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders, 21, 1947 – 1953. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kelley, W. M., Miezin, F. M., McDermott, K. B., Buckner, R. L., Raichle, M. E., Cohen, N. , et al. (1998). Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding. Neuron, 20, 927 – 936. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kida, Y., Tachibana, H., Takeda, M., Yoshikawa, H. & Okita, T. (2007). Recognition memory for unfamiliar faces in Parkinson’s disease: Behavioral and electrophysiologic measures. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 13, 157 – 164. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kirsch-Darrow, L., Marsiske, M., Okun, M. S., Bauer, R. & Bowers, D. (2011). Apathy and depression: Separate factors in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 1058 – 1066. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kriegeskorte, N., Formisano, E., Sorger, B. & Goebel, R. (2007). Individual faces elicit distinct response patterns in human anterior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 104, 20600 – 20605. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laasko, A., Vilkman, H., Bergman, J., Haaparanta, M., Solin, O. & Syvälahti, E. (2002). Sex differences in striatal presynaptic dopamine synthesis capacity in healthy subjects. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 759 – 763. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laux, L., Glanzmann, P., Schaffner, P. & Spielberger, C. D. (1981). “Das State-Trait-Angstinventar (Testmappe mit Handanweisung, Fragebogen STAI-G Form X 1 und Fragebogen STAI-G Form X 2)”. Weinheim: Beltz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Levin, B. E., Llabre, M. M. & Weiner, W. J. (1989). Cognitive impairments associated with early Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 39, 557 – 561. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Locascio, J. J., Corkin, S. & Growdon, J. H. (2003). Relation between clinical characteristics of Parkinson’s disease and cognitive decline. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 94 – 109. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maitland, S. B., Herlitz, A., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L. & Nilsson, L. (2004). Selective sex differences in declarative memory. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1160 – 1169. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Callicot, J. H., Bertolino, A., Goldberg, T. E. & Chase, T. N. (2002). Dopaminergic modulation of cortical function in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 51, 156 – 164. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miller, I. N. & Cronin-Golomb, A. (2010). Gender differences in Parkinson’s disease: clinical characteristics and cognition. Movement Disorders, 25, 2695 – 2703. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Milner, B. (1968). Visual recognition and recall after right temporal-lobe excision in man. Neuropsychologia, 6, 191 – 209. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moscovitch, D. A. & McAndrews, M. P. (2002). Material-specific deficits in “remembering” in patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy and excisions. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1335 – 1342. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mozley, L. H., Gur, R. C., Mozley, P. D. & Gur, R. E. (2001). Striatal dopamine transporters and cognitive functioning in healthy men and women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1492 – 1499. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nilsson, L. (2003). Memory function in normal aging. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 107, 7 – 13. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nilsson, L., Bäckman, L., Erngrund, K., Nyberg, L., Adolfsson, R., Bucht, G. et al. (1997). The Betula prospective cohort study: Memory, health, and aging. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 4, 1 – 32. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Owen, M. A., James, M., Leigh, N. P., Summers, A. B., Marsden, D. C., Quinn, P. N. et al. (1992). Fronto-striatal cognitive deficits at different stages of Parkinson’s disease. Brain, 115, 1727 – 1751. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pavon, J. M., Whitson, H. E. & Okun, M. S. (2010). Parkinson’s disease in women: A call for improved clinical studies and for comparative effectiveness research. Maturitas, 65, 352 – 358. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rasch, B., Friese, M., Hofmann, W. & Naumann, E. (2010). Quantitative Methoden. Einführung in die Statistik für Psychologen und Sozialwissenschaftler. Heidelberg: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rehnman, J. & Herlitz, A. (2007). Women remember more faces than men do. Acta Psychologica, 124, 344 – 355. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riedel, O., Klotsche, J., Spottke, A., Deuschl, G., Förstl, H., Henn, F. et al. (2008). Cognitive impairment in 873 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Results from the German Study on Epidemiology of Parkinson’s Disease with Dementia (GEPAD). Journal of Neurology, 255, 255 – 264. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sambeth, A., Blokland, A., Harmer, C. J., Kilkens, T. O., Nathan, P. J., Porter, R. J. et al. (2007). Sex differences in the effect of acute tryptophan depletion on declarative episodic memory: a pooled analysis of nine studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 516 – 529. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schlaepfer, T. E., Harris, G. J., Tien, A. Y., Peng, L., Lee, S. & Pearlson, G. D. (1995). Structural differences in the cerebral cortex of healthy female and male subjects: A magnetic resonance imaging study. Psychiatry Research, 61, 129 – 135. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt, K. H. & Metzler, P. (1992). Wortschatztest (WST). Weinheim: Beltz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Snodgrass, J. G. & Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 34 – 50. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tam, C. W.C., Burton, J. E., McKeith, G. I., Burn, J. D. & O’Brian, T. J. (2005). Temporal lobe atrophy on MRI in Parkinson’s disease with dementia. A comparison with Alzheimer disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology, 64, 861 – 865. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, A. R., Saint-Cyr, J. A. & Lang, A. E. (1990). Memory and learning in early Parkinson’s disease: Evidence for a “frontal lobe syndrome”. Brain and Cognition, 13, 211 – 232. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tomer, R., Levin, B. E. & Weiner, W. J. (1993). Side of onset of motor symptoms influences cognition in Parkinson’s disease. Annals of Neurology, 34, 579 – 584. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vilsten, J. S. & Mundy, M. E. (2014). Imaging early consolidation of perceptual learning with face stimuli during rest. Brain and Cognition, 85, 170 – 179. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition memory test. Windsor (UK): NFER-Nelson. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wagner, A., Poldrack, R., Eldridge, L., Desmond, J., Glover, G. & Gabrieli, J. (1998). Material-specific lateralization of prefrontal activation during episodic encoding and retrieval. Neuroreport, 9, 3711 – 3717. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weirich, S., Hoffman, F., Meißner, L., Heinz, A. & Bengner, T. (2011). Sex influence on face recognition memory moderated by presentation duration and re-encoding. Neuropsychology, 25, 806 – 813. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Whittington, J. C., Podd, J. & Kan, M. M. (2000). Recognition memory impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Power and meta-analyses. Neuropsychology, 14, 233 – 246. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wittmann, B. C., Schott, B. H., Guderian, S., Frey, J. U., Heinze, H. J. & Duzel, E. (2005). Reward-related FMRI activation of dopaminergic midbrain is associated with enhanced hippocampus-dependent long-term memory formation. Neuron 45, 459 – 467. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wrase, J., Klein, S., Gruesser, S. M., Hermann, D., Flor, H., Mann, K. et al. (2003). Gender differences in the processing of standardized emotional visual stimuli in humans: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 348, 41 – 45. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yagi, S., Yoshikawa, E., Futatsubashi, M., Yokokura, M., Yoshihara, Y., Torizuka, T. et al. (2010). Progression from unilateral to bilateral parkinsonism in early Parkinson disease: Implication of mesocortical dopamine dysfunction by PET. The Journal of Nuclear Medicine, 51, 1250 – 1258. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar