Skip to main content
Original communication

Visual Information Processing in Augmented Reality: Some Aspects of Background Motion

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.63.3.183

We assessed discomfort, time and number of saccades for reading a text superimposed to a moving background. When background moved in the main directions of reading (left to right or top to bottom), discomfort was rated significantly lower than when background moved in the opposite directions. Neither time for reading nor number of saccades are affected by background motion. We therefore conclude that increased arousal might be responsible to cope with effects of background motion, resulting in an increased stress and rising discomfort when background moves opposite to main directions of reading. The findings suggest that reading in Augmented Reality for mobile use must be considered as a robust process. However, load may be increased and cause complaints in a prolonged reading task.

References

  • Abadi, R. , Howard, I. P. , Ohmi, M. (1999). Gaze orientation during full-field and peripheral field passive optokinesis. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 19, 261– 265 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brickenkamp, R. (1994). Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungstest (Test d2) [d2 test of attention]. Göttingen: Hogrefe First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, R. H. S. (1988). The movements of the eyes. London: Pion First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Garbutt, S. , Han, Y. , Kumar, A. R. , Harwood, M. , Harris, C. M. , Leigh, R. J. (2003). Vertical optokinetic nystagmus and saccades in normal human subjects. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 44, 3833– 3841 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grüsser, O. J. , Landis, T. (1991). Visual agnosias and other disturbances of visual perception and cognition. In J. R. Cronly-Dillon (Ed.), Vision and visual dysfunction, (Vol. 13.), Houndmills: MacMillan First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Haitz, P. (1986). Ein Verfahren zur normierten Flimmerbeurteilung von Bildschirmgeräten, basierend auf experimentellen Untersuchungen zur Flimmerverschmelzungsfrequenz des menschlichen Auges [A standardised procedure for assessing flicker of visual display units, basing on experimental results of flicker fusion frequency of the human eye]. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Technische Universität München, Germany First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kurk, R. S. , Muter, P. (1984). Reading of continuous text on video screens. Human Factors, 26, 339– 345 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Menozzi, M. (2000). Visual ergonomics of head-mounted displays. Japanese Psychological Research, 42, 213– 221 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Menozzi, M. , Hofer, F. , Näpflin, U. , Krueger, H. (2003). Visual performance in augmented reality systems for mobile use. In M. Fjeld (Ed.), Augmented reality - usability and collaborative aspects International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, , 16(Special issue), 447– 460 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Menozzi, M. , Koga, K. , Ohta, Y. , Bergande, E. (2003). Evaluation of two algorithms for counting forward saccades in a reading task. Biomedizinische Technik, 48, 176– 181 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Menz, Ch. , Groner, R. (1985). Experiments on visual orientation in reading: effects of lining, letter spacing and inserted elements. In R. Groner, G. W. McConkie, & Ch. Menz. (Eds.), Eye movements and human information processing(pp. 239-250). Amsterdam: Elsevier First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, R. K. , Rayner, K. (1991). Eye movements in skilled reading: Implications for developmental dyslexia. In J. F. Stein (Ed. Vision and visual dysfunction, Vol. 13, 233– 238 Houndmills: Macmillan First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roufs, A. J. , Boschman, M. C. (1991). Visual comfort and performance. In J. A. Roufs (Ed.) Vision and visual dysfunction, Vol. 15, 24– 37 Houndmills: Macmillan First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roufs, A. J. , Boschman, M. C. , Leermakers, M. A. M. (1988). Visual comfort as a criterion for designing display units. In G. C. Van der Veer & G. Mulder (Eds.), Human-Computer Interaction: Psychonomic Aspects(pp. 53-74). Berlin: Springer First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt, D. , Fehr, J. , Bach, M. (2000). Lesegeschwindigkeit bei Bewegung in der Gesichtsfeldperipherie. Ophthalmologe, 97, 846– 848 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Starr, M. S. , Rayner, K. (2001). Eye movements during reading: Some current controversies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 156– 163 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tufano, D. R. (1997). Automotive HUDs: The overlooked safety issues.. Human Factors, 39, 303– 311 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watanabe, K. (2001). Modulation of spatial attention with unidirectional field of motion: An implication for the shift of the OKN beating filed. Vision Research, 41, 801– 814 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar