Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.66.1.51

Various studies have shown that problem solving which requires some kind of transfer can be improved subsequent to knowledge acquisition in pairs (cf. Olivera & Straus, 2004). It has been observed that positive mood may also lead to increased cognitive performance (e.g., Isen, Daubman, & Gorgoglione, 1987). These two approaches were combined in two studies. Results of Experiment 1 show that after learning in dyads and after individual mood induction, individuals in a positive mood were more able to solve transfer tasks than those in a negative mood. In Experiment 2, participants underwent the learning phase alone or in dyads; after mood induction all transfer tasks were solved in pairs. Results indicate that mood induction was the main factor influencing better transfer achievement, whereas learning in dyads only facilitated performance of proximal transfer tasks. Hence, positive mood was the main factor for good transfer performance; whether learning occurred alone or in pairs and whether transfer was performed individually or in dyads is not as important as the mood of a person.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1995). Cognitive psychology and its implications. New York: Freeman First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Andrade, H. , Perkins, D. N. (1998). Learnable intelligence and intelligent learning. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Intelligence, instruction, and assessment: Theory into practice (pp. 67-94). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Argote, L. , Gruenfeld, D. , Naquin, C. (2000). Group learning in organizations. In M. Turner (Ed.), Group at work: Advances in theory and research (pp. 369-411). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bless, H. (2001). Mood and the use of general knowledge structures. In L. L. Martin & G. L. Clore (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook (pp. 9-26). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bodenhausen, G. V. , Kramer, G. P. , Süsser, K. (1994). Happiness and stereo-typic thinking in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 621– 632 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bodenhausen, G. V. , Lichtenstein, M. (1987). Social stereotypes and information processing strategies: Testing process models for stereotype use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 871– 880 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bodenhausen, G. V. , Sheppard, L. A. , Kramer, G. P. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 45– 62 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bohner, G. , Bless, H. , Schwarz, N. , Strack, F. (1988). What triggers causal attributions? The impact of valence and subjective probability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 335– 345 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129– 148 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brand, S. , Reimer, T. , Opwis, K. (2003). Effects of metacognitive thinking and knowledge acquisition in dyads on individual problem solving and transfer performance. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 62, 251– 261 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Brand, S. , Reimer, T. , Opwis, K. (in press). How do we learn in a negative mood? Effects of a negative mood on transfer and learning. Learning and Instruction, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brodbeck, F. C. , Greitemeyer, T. (2000). Effects of individual versus mixed individual and group experience in rule induction on group member learning and group performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 621– 648 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, A. L. , Palincsar, A. S. (1989). Guided, cooperative learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction (pp. 393-451). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Burnstein, E. , Berbaum, M. L. (1983). Stages in group decision making: The decompensation of historical narratives. Political Psychology, 10, 531– 561 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Campione, J. C. , Brown, A. L. , Ferrara, R. A. (1982). Mental retardation and intelligence. In R.J. Steinberg (Ed.), Handbook of human intelligence (pp. 392-490). New York: Cambridge University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cannon-Bowers, J. A. , Salas, E. (1998). Individual and team decision making under stress: Theoretical underpinnings. In J. A. Cannon-Bowers & E. Salas (Eds.), Making decision under stress (pp. 17-38). Washington DC: American Psychological Association First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cannon-Bowers, J. A. , Salas, E. , Converse, S. (1993). Shared mental models in expert team decision making. In N. Castellan (Ed.), Individual and group decision making (pp. 221-246). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752– 766 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Clore, G. L. , Schwarz, N. , Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 323-417). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davidson, J. E. , Deuser, R. , Sternberg, R. J. (1994). The role of metacognition in problem solving. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura (Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing (pp. 207-226). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Derryberry, D. , Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In S. Kitayama & P. M. Niedenthal (Eds.), The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167-196). San Diego, CA: Academic Press First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Detterman, D. K. , Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.) (1993). Transfer on trial: Intelligence, cognition, and instruction. Norwood, NJ: Ablex First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Diener, E. , Diener, C. (1996). Most people are happy. Psychological Science, 7, 181– 185 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunnette, M. D. , Campbell, J. , Jaastad, K. (1963). The effect of group participation on brainstorming effectiveness for two industrial samples. Journal of Applied Psychology, 47, 30– 37 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellis, H. C. , Ashbrook, P. W. (1988). Resource allocation model of the effects of depressed mood states on memory. In K. Fiedler & J. Forgas (Eds.), Affects, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 25-43). Toronto: Hogrefe International First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Estrada, C. A. , Isen, A. M. , Young, M. J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring in reasoning among physicians. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 117– 135 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fogarty, R. , Perkins, D. N. , Barell, J (1992). How to teach for transfer. Palatine, IL: Skylight Publishing First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Forman, E. A. , Larreamendy-Joerns, J. (1995). Learning in the context of peer collaboration: A pluralistic perspective on goals and expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 549 654 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fredrickson, B. L. , Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313– 332 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gasper, K. (2003). When necessity is the mother of invention: Mood and problem solving. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 248– 262 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goodman, P. S , Leyden, D. P. (1991). Familiarity and group productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 578– 586 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gray, W. D. , Orasanu, J. M. (1987). Transfer of cognitive skills. In S. M. Cormier & J. D. Hagman (Eds.), Transfer of learning: Contemporary research and applications (pp. 183-215). New York: Academic Press First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greene, T. R. , Noice, H. (1988). Influence of positive affect upon creative thinking and problem solving in children. Psychological Report, 63, 895– 89 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hollingshead, A. B. (1998). Group and individual training: The impact of practice on performance. Small Group Research, 29, 254– 280 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hudgins, B. B. (1960). Effects of group experience on individual problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 51, 37– 42 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isbell, L. (2003). Not all happy people are lazy or stupid: Evidence of systematic processing in happy moods. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 341– 349 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 179-236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 203-253). San Diego: Academic Press First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. , Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206– 1217 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. , Daubman, K. A. , Gorgoglione, J. M. (1987). The influence of positive affect on cognitive organization: Implications for education. In R. Snow & M. Farr (Eds.), Aptitude, Learning and Instruction. Volume 3: Conative and Affective Process Analyses (pp. 143-167). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. , Daubman, K. A. , Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122– 1131 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. , Labroo, A. A. (2003). Some ways in which positive affect facilitates decision making and judgement. In. S. L. Schneider & J. Shanteau (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on judgement and decision research (pp. 365-393). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. , Rosenzweig, A. S. , Young, M. J. (1991). The influence of positive affect on clinical problem solving. Medical Decision Making, 11, 221– 227 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. , Byrne, R. M. J. , Schaeken, W. (1992). Propositional reasoning by model. Psychological Review, 99, 418– 439 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Katona, G. (1940). Organizing and memorizing. New York. Columbia University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kotovsky, K. , Fallside, D (1989). Representation and transfer in problem solving. In D. Klar & K. Kotovsky (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 69-108). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kotovsky, K. , Hayes, J. R. , Simon, H. E. (1985). Why are some problems hard? Evidence from the Tower of Hanoi. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 248– 294 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Larkin, J. H. (1989). What kind of knowledge transfers?. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 283-306). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Laughlin, P. R. , Barth, J. M. (1981). Group-to-individual and individual-to-group in problem solving transfer. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 1087– 1093 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laughlin, P. R. , Ellis, A. L. (1986). Demonstrability and social combination processes on mathematical intellective tasks. Journal of Eperimental Social Psychology, 22, 177– 189 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laughlin, P. R. , Sweeney, J. D. (1977). Individual-to-Group and Group-to-Individual transfer problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 246– 254 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, A. Y. , Sternthal, B. (1999). The effects of positive mood on memory. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 115– 127 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Littlepage, G. , Robison, W. , Reddington, K. (1997). Effects of task experience and group experience on group performance, member ability, and recognition of expertise. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 133– 147 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mackie, D. M. , Worth, L. T. (1989). Cognitive deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 27– 40 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGlynn, R. P. , Schick, C. (1973). Dyadic concept attainment as a function of interaction format, memory requirements, and sex. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 335– 340 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood: The frame of mind. New York: Springer First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nowlis, V. , Nowlis, H. H. (1956). The description and analysis of mood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 65, 345– 355 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oaksford, M. , Morris, F. , Grainger, B. , Williams, J. M. (1996). Mood, reasoning, and central executive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 476– 492 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Olivera, F. , Straus, S. G. (2004). Group-to-individual transfer of learning: Cognitive and social factors. Small Group Research, 35, 440– 465 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Opwis, K. (1996). Reflexionen über eigenes und fremdes Wissen [Reflexions about one’s own and the others knowledge].. In F. Klix & H. Spada (Hrsg.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie. Themenbereich C: Theorie und Forschung. (S. 269-401). Serie II: Kognition. Band 6: Wissenspsychologie. Göttingen: Hogrefe First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Perlmutter, H. V. , deMontmollin, G. (1952). Group learning of nonsense syllables. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 762– 769 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1986a). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 124-203). New York: Academic Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1986b). Communication and persuasion: central and peripherical routes to attitude change. New York: Springer First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reimer, T. (2001). Attributions for poor group performance as a predictor of perspective-taking and subsequent group achievement: A process model. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 4, 31– 47 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roehm, M. L. , Sternthal, B. (2001). The moderating effect of knowledge and resources on the persuasive impact of analogies. Journal of Consumer Research, 28, 257– 272 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmid, U. , Wysotzki, F. (1998). Induction of recursive program schemes. Proceedings of the 10th European Conference on Machine Learning (ECML-98), Chemnitz, Germany, 21– 225 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwartz, D. L. (1995). The emergence of abstract representations in dyad problem solving. The Journal of the Learning Science, 4, 321– 354 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and Cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527-561). New York: Guilford First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarz, N. , Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgements of well-being; informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513– 523 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sinclair, R. C. (1988). Mood, categorization breadth, and performance appraisal: The effects of order of information acquisition and affective state on halo, accuracy, information retrieval, and evaluations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 42, 22– 46 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sinclair, R. C. , Mark, M. M. (1992). The influence of mood state on judgement and action: Effects on persuasion, categorization, social justice, person perception, and judgmental accuracy. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgment (pp.165-193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Singley, M. , Anderson, J. R. (1989). The transfer of cognitive skill. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Steiner, G. (1996). Lernverhalten, Lernleistung und Instruktionsmethoden [Learning behavior, performance and methods of instructions]. In F. E. Weinert (Hrsg.), Psychologie des Lernens und der Instruktion. Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Themenbereich D: Praxisgebiete, Serie I, Pädagogische Psychologie, Bd. 2 (pp. 279-317). Göttingen: Hogrefe First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, D. W. , Faust, W. L. (1952). Twenty questions: Efficiency in problem solving as a function of size of group. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 360– 368 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • VanLehn, K. (1996). Cognitive skill acquisition. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 513– 539 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weary, G. , Jacobsen, J. A. (1997). Causal uncertainty beliefs and diagnostic information seeking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 150– 153 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weary, G. , Marsh, K. L. , Gleicher, F. , Edwards, J. A. (1993). Depression, control motivation and the processing of information about others. In G. Weary, G. Gleicher & K. L. Marsh (Eds.), Control motivation and social cognition (pp. 255-287). New York: Springer First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webb, N. M. , Palincsar, A. S. (1996). Group processes in the classroom. In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (3rd ed., pp. 841-873). New York: Macmillan First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wegner, D. M. , Erber, R. , Raymond, P. (1991). Transactive memory in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 923– 929 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegener, D. T. , Petty, R. E. , Smith, S. M. (1995). Positive mood can increase or decrease message scrutiny: The hedonic contingency view of mood and message processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 5– 15 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westermann, R. , Spies, K. , Stahl, G. , Hesse, F. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557– 580 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar