Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.68.1.17

In the Geneva Variability Study, the Stroop task was administered to children and young adults. Interference and facilitation effects were investigated by comparing mean reaction times (RTs) and applying ex-Gaussian distribution analysis. Our analyses were motivated by three goals: First, we aimed to replicate the results obtained with young adults by Spieler, Balota, and Faust (2000); second, we applied this method to children’s data; third, the question of age-related differences in these inhibitory processes was investigated with the ex-Gaussian approach. In young adults, findings pertaining to the interference effect reported by Spieler et al. (2000) were replicated; in children, the interference effect was only manifested in the estimated Gaussian parameters (Mean RT, μ, σ); facilitation effects were only observed in children. The substantive meaning of the results and the advantages of ex-Gaussian analyses for describing RT distributions are discussed.

References

  • Adleman, N. E., Menon, V., Blasey, C. M., White, C. D., Arsofsky, I. S., Glover, G. H., et al. (2002). A developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color-word task. NeuroImage, 16, 61– 75 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, S., Cousineau, D., Heathcote, A. (2001). Technical manual for QMPE v2.18:. Fortran code to fit response time distributions First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, S., Heathcote, A. (2003). QMLE: Fast, robust, and efficient estimation of distribution functions based on quantiles. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 485– 492 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. M. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37, 715– 730 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 332– 361 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Comalli, P., Wapner, S., Werner, H. (1962). Interference effects of Stroop color-word test in childhood, adulthood, and aging. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 100, 47– 53 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coyle, T. R. (2003). A review of the worst performance rule: Evidence, theory, and alternative hypotheses. Intelligence, 31, 567– 587. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dempster, F. N. (1991). Inhibitory processes: A neglected dimension of intelligence. Intelligence, 15, 157–173. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dempster, F. N. (1992). The rise and fall of the inhibitory mechanism: Toward a unified theory of cognitive development and aging. Developmental Review, 12, 45– 75 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Durston, S., Thomas, K. M., Yang, Y., Ulug, A. M., Zimmerman, R. D., Casey, B. J. (2002). A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Developmental Science, 5, 9– 16 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: Performance of children 31/2-7-years-old on a Stroop-like day-night test. Cognition, 53, 129– 153 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hasher, L., Lustig, C., Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory mechanisms and the control of attention. In A. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 227-249). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., Mewhort, D. J. K. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: An example using the Stroop task. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 340– 347 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hockley, W. E. (1984). Analysis of response time distributions in the study of cognitive processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 6, 598– 615 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hohle, R. H. (1965). Inferred components of reaction times as functions of foreperiod duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 382– 386 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huynh, H., Feldt, L. S. (1976). Estimation of the Box correction for degrees of freedom from sample data in randomized block and split-plot designs. Journal of Educational Statistics, 1, 69– 82 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kane, M. J., Engle, R. W. (2003). Working memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 47– 70 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leth-Steensen, C., Elbaz, Z. K., Douglas, V. I. (2000). Mean response times, variability, and skew in the responding of ADHD children: A response time distributional approach. Acta Psychologica, 104, 167– 190 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacLeod, C. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163– 203 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McAuley, T., Yap, S., Christ, S. E., White, D. A. (2006). Revisiting inhibitory control across the life span: Insights from the ex-Gaussian distribution. Developmental Neuropsychology, 29, 447– 458 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGill, W. J. (1963). Stochastic latency mechanisms. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of Mathematical Psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446– 461 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 461– 179 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., Faust, M. E. (2000). Levels of selective attention revealed through analyses of response time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 506– 526 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 18, 643– 662 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I. M., Stuss, D. T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain and Cognition, 49, 402– 419 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J., Logan, G. D., Tannock, R. (1999). Development of inhibitory control across the lifespan. Developmental Psychology, 35, 205– 213 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar