Skip to main content
Original Communication

The Impact of Mood on Affect Regulation

Effects on Film Preferences

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000071

Drawing on the mood-behavior model (Gendolla, 2000), I predicted that both negative and positive moods evoke a stronger need for hedonic affect regulation than a so-called neutral mood. To test this hypothesis, participants were induced into a positive, neutral, or negative mood by autobiographical recollection and then selected which of three films they wanted to watch. The films varied in the extent of their potential for hedonic affect regulation. As expected, preferences for a pleasant film were higher in both positive and negative moods than in a neutral mood and the positive and negative mood conditions did not differ. Furthermore, a regression analysis found that the preference for a pleasant film was related to mood intensity. Implications for other models of affect regulation are discussed.

References

  • Camilli, G., Hopkins, K. D. (1978). Applicability of χ² to 2 × 2 contingency tables with small expected cell frequencies. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 163–167. doi 10.1037/0033–2909.85.1.163 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, M. R. (1988). What do you do when you’re happy or blue? Mood, expectancies, and behavioral interest. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 309–331. doi 10.1007/BF00992357 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Erber, R., Wegner, D. M., Therriault, N. (1996). On being cool and collected: Mood regulation in anticipation of social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 757–766. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.70.4.757 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Forest, D., Clark, M., Mills, J., Isen, A. M. (1979). Helping as a function of feeling state and nature of the helping behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 3, 161–169. doi 10.1007/BF01650601 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gendolla, G. H. E. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior: An integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychology, 4, 378–408. doi 10.1037/1089-2680.4.4.378 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., Abele, A. E., Krüsken, J. (2001). The informational impact of mood on effort mobilization: A study of cardiovascular and electrodermal responses. Emotion, 1, 12–24. doi 10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.12 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., Brinkmann, K. (2005). The role of mood states in self-regulation: Effects on action preferences and resource mobilization. European Psychologist, 10, 187–198. doi 10.1027/1016-9040.10.3.187 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Gendolla, G. H. E., Krüsken, J. (2002). Mood, task demand, and effort-related cardiovascular response. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 577–603. doi 10.1080/02699930143000446 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299. doi 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Handley, I. M., Lassiter, G. D. (2002). Mood and information processing: When happy and sad looks the same. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 223–255. doi 10.1023/A:1021725130325 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300. doi 10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 417–435). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Isen, A. M., Simmonds, S. F. (1978). The effect of feeling good on a helping task that is incompatible with good mood. Social Psychology Quarterly, 41, 345–349. doi 10.2307/3033588 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klinger, E. (1982). On the self-management of mood, affect, and attention. In P. Karoly, F. Kanfer (Eds.), Self-management and behavior change: From theory to practice (pp. 129–164). New York, NY: Pergamon. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Koole, S. L. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 4–41. doi 10.1080/02699930802619031 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Larsen, R. J. (2000). Toward a science of mood regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 129–141. doi 10.1207/S15327965PLI1103_01 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Matthews, G., Jones, D. M., Chamberlain, A. G. (1990). Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST mood adjective checklist. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 17–42. doi 10.1111/j.2044–8295.1990.tb02343.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, W. N. (1989). Mood: The frame of mind. New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, W. N., Reilly, N. P. (1987). Toward the self-regulation of mood: Theory and research. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 215–249. doi 10.1007/BF01001412 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parkinson, B., Totterdell, P. (1999). Classifying affect-regulation strategies. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 277–303. doi 10.1080/026999399379221 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parkinson, B., Totterdell, P., Briner, R. B., Reynolds, S. (1996). Changing moods: The psychology of mood and mood regulation. London, UK: Longman. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Parrott, W. G. (1993). Beyond hedonism: Motives for inhibiting good moods and for maintaining bad moods. In D. M. Wegner, J. W. Pennebaker (Eds.), Handbook of mental control (pp. 278–305). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Posner, J., Russell, J. A., Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 715–734. doi 10.1017/ S0954579405050340 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K. (2011). Counterregulation and control-dependency. Social Psychology, 42, 56–66. doi 10.1027/1864-9335/ a000043 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Saavedra, R., Earley, P. C. (1991). Choice of task and goal under conditions of general and specific affective inducement. Motivation and Emotion, 15, 45–65. doi 10.1007/BF00991475 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schaller, M., Cialdini, R. B. (1990). Happiness, sadness, and helping. In E. T. Higgins, R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 265–296). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Silvestrini, N., Gendolla, G. H. E. (2007). Mood effects on autonomic activity in mood regulation. Psychophysiology, 44, 650–659. doi 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00532.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silvestrini, N., Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009a). The joint effect of mood, task valence, and task difficulty on effort-related cardiovascular response and facial EMG. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73, 226–234. doi 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silvestrini, N., Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009b). Mood-regulative hedonic incentive interacts with mood and task difficulty to determine effort-related cardiovascular response and facial EMG. Biological Psychology, 82, 54–63. doi 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.05.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Solomon, R. L., Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119–145. doi 10.1037/h0036128 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sommers, S. (1984). Reported emotions and conventions of emotionality among college students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 207–215. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.46.1.207 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 101–105. doi 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01617.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamir, M., Chiu, C.-Y., Gross, J. (2007). Business or pleasure? Utilitarian versus hedonic considerations in emotion regulation. Emotion, 7, 546–554. doi 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.546 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilization-minimization hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 67–85. doi 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.67 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Thayer, R. E. (1996). The origin of everyday moods. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Thayer, R. E., Newman, J. R., McClain, T. M. (1994). Self-regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, raising energy, and reducing tension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 910–925. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.67.5.910 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 53–67. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.53 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E. (1994). Mood management across affective states: The hedonic contingency hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1034–1048. doi 10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.1034 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wyer, R. S., Clore, G. L., Isbell, L. M. (1999). Affect and information processing. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 31, 1–77. doi 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60271-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zillmann, D. (1988). Mood management: Using entertainment to full advantage. In L. Donohew, H. E. Snyder, E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Communication, social cognition, and affect (pp. 147–171). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar