Skip to main content
Themenschwerpunkt

Das Negativsymptom Apathie

Eine Folge dysfunktionaler Entscheidungsfindung?

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1661-4747/a000299

Zusammenfassung. Die Apathie ist eine Dimension von Negativsymptomen, die zu erheblichen Beeinträchtigungen in der Alltagsfunktion führt, ohne dass adäquate Behandlungsverfahren zur Verfügung stehen. Die Apathie wird heute zunehmend als eine Reduktion von zielgerichtetem Verhalten definiert. Die dem zielgerichteten Verhalten zugrundeliegenden Prozesse lassen sich in einem Modell verstehen, das verschiedene Phasen der Entscheidungsfindung beinhaltet. Hierzu gehören die Generierung von Handlungsoptionen, die Bewertung und Auswahl von Handlungsoptionen und schliesslich das Lernen aus Handlungsergebnissen. Diese Prozesse und ihre Dysfunktionen lassen sich quantitativ erfassen, wie wir am Beispiel von Studien bei Patienten mit Schizophrenie zeigen. Dysfunktionen in allen genannten Entscheidungsphasen sind mit der Apathie assoziiert. Dieses Modell kann dazu dienen, die Entwicklung psychologischer und biologischer Therapieverfahren gezielter auf die zugrundeliegenden dysfunktionalen Prozesse aufzubauen.


The negative symptom apathy – a consequence of dysfunctional decision-making?

Abstract. Apathy is a dimension of negative symptoms that leads to significant impairment in everyday functioning. At the same time there are no adequate treatment options. Recently, apathy has been defined as a reduction of goal-directed behavior. The processes subserving goal-directed behavior can be understood with a model that includes sequential stages of decision making. These stages consist of generation of options for action, valuation and selection of options and learning from action outcomes. These processes and their dysfunctions can be measured quantitatively as is shown in recent studies including patients with schizophrenia. Dysfunctions in all three stages of decision making are associated with apathy. This model can be employed to target psychological and biological interventions more specifically at the underlying dysfunctional processes.

Literatur

  • Aleman, A. , Lincoln, T. M. , Bruggeman, R. , Melle, I. , Arends, J. , Arango, C. & Knegtering, H. (2016). Treatment of negative symptoms: Where do we stand, and where do we go? Schizophrenia Research (im Druck). First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Andreasen, N. C. (1982). Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Definition and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry , 39 , 784–788. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, R. G. & Pluck, G. (2000). Negative symptoms: the ‘pathology’ of motivation and goal-directed behaviour. Trends in Neuroscience , 23 , 412–417. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Collins, A. G. , Brown, J. K. , Gold, J. M. , Waltz, J. A. & Frank, M. J. (2014). Working memory contributions to reinforcement learning impairments in schizophrenia. Journal of Neuroscience , 34 , 13747–13756. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Favrod, J. , Nguyen, A. , Fankhauser, C. , Ismailaj, A. , Hasler, J. D. , Ringuet, A. , … Bonsack, C. (2015). Positive Emotions Program for Schizophrenia (PEPS): a pilot intervention to reduce anhedonia and apathy. BMC Psychiatry , 15 , 231. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gold, J. M. , Waltz, J. A. , Matveeva, T. M. , Kasanova, Z. , Strauss, G. P. , Herbener, E. S. , … Frank, M. J. (2012). Negative symptoms and the failure to represent the expected reward value of actions: behavioral and computational modeling evidence. Archives of General Psychiatry , 69 , 129–138. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hartmann, M. N. , Hager, O. M. , Reimann, A. V. , Chumbley, J. R. , Kirschner, M. , Seifritz, E. , … Kaiser, S. (2015). Apathy but not diminished expression in schizophrenia is associated with discounting of monetary rewards by physical effort. Schizophrenia Bulletin , 41 , 503–512. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hartmann, M. N. , Kluge, A. , Kalis, A. , Mojzisch, A. , Tobler, P. N. & Kaiser, S. (2015). Apathy in schizophrenia as a deficit in the generation of options for action. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 124 , 309–318. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaiser, S. , Lyne, J. , Agartz, I. , Clarke, M. , Morch-Johnsen, L. & Faerden, A. (2016). Individual negative symptoms and domains – Relevance for assessment, pathomechanisms and treatment. Schizophrenia Research (im Druck). First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaiser, S. , Simon, J. J. , Kalis, A. , Schweizer, S. , Tobler, P. N. & Mojzisch, A. (2013). The cognitive and neural basis of option generation and subsequent choice. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience , 13 , 814–829. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kalis, A. , Kaiser, S. & Mojzisch, A. (2013). Why we should talk about option generation in decision-making research. Frontiers in Psychology , 4 , 555. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kalis, A. , Mojzisch, A. , Schweizer, T. S. & Kaiser, S. (2008). Weakness of will, akrasia, and the neuropsychiatry of decision making: an interdisciplinary perspective. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience , 8 , 402–417. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kanfer, F. H. & Saslow, G. (1965). Behavioral analysis: An alternative to diagnostic classification. Archives of General Psychiatry , 12 , 529–538. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Levy, R. & Dubois, B. (2006). Apathy and the functional anatomy of the prefrontal cortex-basal ganglia circuits. Cerebral Cortex , 16 , 916–928. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lindenmayer, J. P. , Nasrallah, H. , Pucci, M. , James, S. & Citrome, L. (2013). A systematic review of psychostimulant treatment of negative symptoms of schizophrenia: challenges and therapeutic opportunities. Schizophrenia Research , 147 , 241–252. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Messinger, J. W. , Tremeau, F. , Antonius, D. , Mendelsohn, E. , Prudent, V. , Stanford, A. D. & Malaspina, D. (2011). Avolition and expressive deficits capture negative symptom phenomenology: implications for DSM-5 and schizophrenia research. Clinical and Psychological Review , 31 , 161–168. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pessiglione, M. , Seymour, B. , Flandin, G. , Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. (2006). Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature , 442 , 1042–1045. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reddy, L. F. , Horan, W. P. , Barch, D. M. , Buchanan, R. W. , Dunayevich, E. , Gold, J. M. , . . . Green, M. F. (2015). Effort-based decision-making paradigms for clinical trials in schizophrenia: part 1-psychometric characteristics of 5 paradigms. Schizophrenia Bulletin , 41 , 1045–1054. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Revheim, N. , Schechter, I. , Kim, D. , Silipo, G. , Allingham, B. , Butler, P. & Javitt, D. C. (2006). Neurocognitive and symptom correlates of daily problem-solving skills in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research , 83 , 237–245. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenberg, P. B. , Lanctot, K. L. , Drye, L. T. , Herrmann, N. , Scherer, R. W. , Bachman, D. L. , … Investigators, A. (2013). Safety and efficacy of methylphenidate for apathy in Alzheimer's disease: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry , 74 , 810–816. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strauss, G. P. , Frank, M. J. , Waltz, J. A. , Kasanova, Z. , Herbener, E. S. & Gold, J. M. (2011). Deficits in positive reinforcement learning and uncertainty-driven exploration are associated with distinct aspects of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry , 69 , 424–431. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Schaaf, M. E. , van Schouwenburg, M. R. , Geurts, D. E. , Schellekens, A. F. , Buitelaar, J. K. , Verkes, R. J. & Cools, R. (2014). Establishing the dopamine dependency of human striatal signals during reward and punishment reversal learning. Cerebral Cortex , 24 , 633–642. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Velligan, D. I. , Roberts, D. , Mintz, J. , Maples, N. , Li, X. , Medellin, E. & Brown, M. (2015). A randomized pilot study of MOtiVation and Enhancement (MOVE) Training for negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research , 165 , 175–180. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Waltz, J. A. , Frank, M. J. , Robinson, B. M. & Gold, J. M. (2007). Selective reinforcement learning deficits in schizophrenia support predictions from computational models of striatal-cortical dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry , 62 , 756–764. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wardle, M. C. , Treadway, M. T. , Mayo, L. M. , Zald, D. H. & de Wit, H. (2011). Amping up effort: effects of d-amphetamine on human effort-based decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience , 31 , 16597–16602. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weber, S. C. , Beck-Schimmer, B. , Kajdi, M. E. , Muller, D. , Tobler, P. N. & Quednow, B. B. (2016). Dopamine D2/3- and mu-opioid receptor antagonists reduce cue-induced responding and reward impulsivity in humans. Translational Psychiatry , 6 , e850. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wykes, T. , Huddy, V. , Cellard, C. , McGurk, S. R. & Czobor, P. (2011). A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. American Journal of Psychiatry , 168 , 472–485. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar