Skip to main content
Short Research Report

Dissociable Age Effects in Focus-Switching

Out of Sight, Out of Mind

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000034

Focus-switching has recently been identified as an executive control process with differential age sensitivity. To date, the assumption of dissociable age effects is based on only two kinds of tasks constricting its conclusiveness. In a study with 85 younger (19–35 years) and 91 older adults (59–80 years), age effects were again dissociable in two alternative tasks with respect to the availability and the accessibility of representations. The results validate earlier findings that focus-switching is primarily affected in maintaining representations rather than in accessing them.

References

  • Anderson, J. R., Reder, L. M., Llebiere, C. (1996). Working memory: Activation limitations on retrieval. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 221–256. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Basak, C., Verhaeghen, P. (in press). Three layers of working memory: Focus-switch costs and retrieval dynamics as revealed by the N-count task. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cerella, J. (1994). Generalized slowing in Brinley plots. Journal of Gerontology, 49, 65–71. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cowan, N. (1997). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cowan, N. (2005). Working memory capacity. New York: Taylor & Francis. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dorbath, L., Hasselhorn, M., Titz, C. (submitted). Aging and executive functioning: A training study on focus-switching. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Garavan, H. (1998). Serial attention within working memory. Memory and Cognition, 26, 263–276. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Garavan, H., Ross, T. J., Li, S. J., Stein, E. A. (2000). A parametric manipulation of central executive functioning. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 585–592. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and new review. In G. H. Bower, (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kray, J., Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task-switching. Psychology and Aging, 15, 126–147. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kray, J., Eppinger, B., Mecklinger, A. (2005). Age differences in attentional control: An event-related potential approach. Psychophysiology, 42, 407–416. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mayr, U., Spieler, D. H., Kliegl, R. (2001). Ageing and executive control. New York: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McElree, B. (2001). Working memory and focal attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology/Learning, Memory and Cognition, 27, 817–835. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oberauer, K. (2002). Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology/Learning, Memory & Cognition, 28, 411–421. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oberauer, K. (2003). Selective attention to elements in working memory. Experimental Psychology, 50, 257–269. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Oberauer, K., Wendland, M., Kliegl, R. (2003). Age differences in working memory – The roles of storage and selective access. Memory & Cognition, 31, 563–569. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ryan, J. D., Leung, G., Turk-Browne, N. B., Hasher, L. (2007). Assessment of age-related changes in inhibition and binding using eye movement monitoring. Psychology and Aging, 22, 239–250. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt, K.-H., Metzler, P. (1992). Wortschatztest (WST) [Vocabulary test]. Weinheim: Beltz Test. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmiedek, F., Hildebrandt, A., Lövden, M., Wilhelm, O., Lindenberger, U. (2009). Complex span versus updating tasks of working memory: The gap is not that deep. Journal of Experimental Psychology/Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1089–1096. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schönknecht, P., Pantel, J., Kruse, A., Schröder, J. (2005). Prevalence and natural course of aging-associated cognitive decline in a population based sample of “young-old” subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 2071–2077. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Titz, C., Behrendt, J., Hasselhorn, M. (2010). Tomatoes and apples or red and green lines: Are age-related interference effects based on competition among concepts or percepts? Experimental Aging Research, 36, 346–358. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Titz, C., Behrendt, J., Menge, U., Hasselhorn, M. (2008). A reassessment of negative priming within the inhibition framework of cognitive aging: There is more in it than previously believed. Experimental Aging Research, 34, 340–366. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unsworth, N., Engle, R. W. (2008). Speed and accuracy of accessing information in working memory: An individual differences investigation of focus switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology/Learning, Memory & Cognition, 34, 616–630. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Gerven, P. W. M., Meijer, W. A., Jolles, J. (2007). Education does not protect against age-related decline of switching focal attention in working memory. Brain and Cognition, 64, 158–163. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Gerven, P. W. M., Meijer, W. A., Prickaerts, J. H. M., van der Veen, F. M. (2008). Aging and focus switching in working memory: Excluding the potential role of memory load. Experimental Aging Research, 34, 367–378. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vaughan, L., Basak, C., Hartman, M., Verhaeghen, P. (2008). Aging and working memory inside and outside the focus of attention: Dissociations of availability and accessibility. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 15(6), 1–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P., Basak, C. (2005). Ageing and switching of the focus of attention in working memory: Results from a modified N-Back task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 58(1), 134–154. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P., Hoyer, W. J. (2007). Aging, focus switching, and task-switching in a continuous calculation task: Evidence toward a new working memory control process. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14(1), 22–39. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., Basak, C. (2004). A working memory workout: How to expand the focus of serial attention from one to four items in 10 hours or less. Journal of Experimental Psychology/Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30, 1322–1337. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., Basak, C., Bopp, K. L., Zhang, Y., Hoyer, W. J. (2007). The ins and outs of working memory: Dynamic processes associated with focus switching and search. In N. Osaka R. LogieEds., Working memory: Behavioral and neural correlates (pp. 81–98). Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., Bopp, K. L., Basak, C. (2005). Aging and varieties of cognitive control: A review of meta-analyses on resistance to interference, coordination and task-switching, and an experimental exploration of age-sensitivity in the newly identified process of focus switching. In R. W. Engle G. Sedek U. von D. N. McIntoshEds., Cognitive limitations in aging and psychopathology (pp. 160–189). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Verhaeghen, P., Kliegl, R., Mayr, U. (1997). Sequential and coordinative complexity in time – Accuracy functions for mental arithmetic. Psychology and Aging, 12, 555–564. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Voigt, S., Hagendorf, H. (2002). The role of task context for component processes in focus switching. Psychologische Beiträge, 44, 248–274. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar