Skip to main content
Full-Length Research Report

Cognitive Inconsistency and Practice-Related Learning in Older Adults

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000096

The current study examined predictors of individual differences in the magnitude of practice-related improvements achieved by 87 older adults (mean age 63.52 years) over 18 weeks of cognitive practice. Cognitive inconsistency, as measured in both baseline trial-to-trial reaction times and week-to-week accuracy scores, was included as a predictor of practice-related gains in two measures of processing speed. Conditional growth models revealed that both reaction time and accuracy level, as well as rate-of-change in functioning, were related to inconsistency, even after controlling for mean-level, but that increased inconsistency was negatively associated with accuracy versus positively associated with reaction time improvement. Cognitive inconsistency may signal dysregulation in the ability to control cognitive performance or may be indicative of adaptive attempts at functioning.

References

  • Aiken Morgan, A. (2008). Effects of improved physical fitness on cognitive/psychosocial functioning in community-dwelling, sedentary middle-aged and older adults (unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Allaire, J. C., Marsiske, M. (2005). Intraindividual variability may not always indicate vulnerability in elders’ cognitive performance. Psychology and Aging, 20, 390–401. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ball, K., Berch, D. B., Helmers, K. F., Jobe, J. B., Leveck, M. D., Marsiske, M., Morris, J. N. , ... Willis, S. L. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, 2271–2281. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B., Dittmann-Kohli, F., Kliegl, R. (1986). Reserve capacity of the elderly in aging-sensitive tests of fluid intelligence: Replication and extension. Psychology and Aging, 1, 172–177. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B., Kliegl, R., Dittmann-Kohli, F. (1988). On the locus of training gains in research on the plasticity of fluid intelligence in old age. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 392–400. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P. B., Sowarka, D., Kliegl, R. (1989). Cognitive training research on fluid intelligence in old age: What can older adults achieve by themselves? Psychology and Aging, 4, 217–221. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brandt, J., Spencer, M., Folstein, M. (1988). The Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 1, 111–117. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bryk, A. S., Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buman, M. P. (2008). Evaluation of a peer-assisted social cognitive physical activity intervention for older adults (unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buman, M. P., Giacobbi, P. R., Dzierzewski, J. M., Aiken Morgan, A., McCrae, C. S., Roberts, B. L., Marsiske, M. (2011). Peer volunteers improve long-term maintenance of physical activity with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 8(Suppl 2), S257–S266. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bunce, D., MacDonald, S. W. S., Hultsch, D. F. (2004). Inconsistency in serial choice decision and motor reaction times dissociate in younger and older adults. Brain and Cognition, 56, 320–327. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burton, C. L., Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E., Hunter, M. A. (2002). Intraindividual variability in physical and emotional functioning: Comparison of adults with traumatic brain injuries and healthy adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16, 264–279. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eschen, A. (2012). The contributions of cognitive trainings to the stability of cognitive, everyday, and brain functioning across adulthood. GeroPsych, 25, 223–234. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Godin, G., Jobin, J., Bouilon, J. (1986). Assessment of leisure time exercise behavior by self-report: A concurrent validity study. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 77, 359–361. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9, 1–65. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofland, B. F., Willis, S. L., Baltes, P. B. (1981). Fluid intelligence performance in the elderly: Intraindividual variability and conditions of assessment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 573–586. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hultsch, D. F., MacDonald, S. W. S., Dixon, R. A. (2002). Variability in reaction time performance of younger and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, P101–PP115. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hultsch, D. F., MacDonald, S. W. S., Hunter, M. A., Levy-Bencheton, J., Strauss, E. (2000). Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in older adults: Comparison of adults with mild dementia, adults with arthritis, and healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 14, 588–598. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E., Hunter, M. A., MacDonald, S. W. S. (2008). Intraindividual variability, cognition, and aging. In F. I. M. Craik T. A. SalthouseEds., The handbook of aging and cognition (3rd ed., pp. 491–556). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 6829–6833. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jarvis, B. G. (2008a). DirectRT (Version 2008). New York: Empirisoft. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jarvis, B. G. (2008b). MediaLab (Version 2008). New York: Empirisoft. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Li, S. C., Aggen, S. H., Nesselroade, J. R., Baltes, P. B. (2001). Short-term fluctuations in elderly people’s sensorimotor functioning predict text and spatial memory performance: The MacArthur Successful Aging Studies. Gerontology, 47, 100–116. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Li, S. C., Huxhold, O., Schmiedek, F. (2004). Aging and attenuated processing robustness. Gerontology, 50, 28–34. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Little, T. D., Bovaird, J. A., Widaman, K. F. (2006). On the merits of orthogonalizing powered and product terms: Implications for modeling interactions among latent variables. Structural Equation Modeling, 13, 497–519. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lustig, C., Shah, P., Seidler, R., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). Aging, training, and the brain: A review and future directions. Neuropsychology Review, 19, 504–522. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacDonald, S. W. S., Hultsch, D. F., Dixon, R. A. (2008). Predicting impending death: Inconsistency in speed is a selective and early marker. Psychology and Aging, 23, 595–607. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McArdle, J. J., Prindle, J. J. (2008). A latent change score analysis of a randomized clinical trial in reasoning training. Psychology and Aging, 23, 702–719. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McAuley, E., Kramer, A. F., Colcombe, S. J. (2004). Cardiovascular fitness and neurocognitive function in older adults: A brief review. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 18, 214–220. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McCoy, K. J. M. (2004). Understanding the transition from normal cognitive aging to mild cognitive impairment: Comparing the intraindividual variability in cognitive function (unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nesselroade, J. R. (1991). The warp and woof of the developmental fabric. In R. Downs L. Liben D. PalermoEds., Visions of development, the environment, and esthetics: The legacy of Joachim F. Wohlwill (pp. 213–240). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nesselroade, J. R., Salthouse, T. A. (2004). Methodological and theoretical implications of intraindividual variability in perceptual-motor performance. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59, P49–PP55. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Noack, H., Lövdén, M., Schmiedek, F., Lindenberger, U. (2009). Cognitive plasticity in adulthood and old age: Gauging the generality of cognitive intervention effects. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 27, 435–453. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee . (2008). Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee Report, 2008. Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Health and Human Services. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Salthouse, T. A., Nesselroade, J. R., Berish, D. E. (2006). Short-term variability in cognitive performance and the calibration of longitudinal change. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61, P144–PP151. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Salthouse, T. A., Schroeder, D. H., Ferrer, E. (2004). Estimating retest effects in longitudinal assessments of cognitive functioning in adults between 18 and 60 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 813–822. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Siegler, R. S. (1994). Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 1–5. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Siegler, R. S. (2007). Cognitive variability. Developmental Science, 10, 104–109. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Siegler, R. S., Lemaire, P. (1997). Older and younger adults’ strategy choices in multiplication: Testing prediction of ASCM using the choice/no-choice method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 71–92. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Singer, J. D., Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test – Revised: Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sliwinski, M. J., Hoffman, L., Hofer, S. M. (2010). Modeling retest and aging effects in a measurement burst design. In P. Molenaar K. M. NewelEds., Individual pathways of change: Statistical models for analyzing learning and development (pp. 37–50). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Geert, P., Van Dijk, M. (2002). Focus on variability: New tools to understand intraindividual variability in developmental data. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 340–374. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Willis, S. L., Schaie, K. W. (2009). Cognitive training and plasticity: Theoretical perspective and methodological consequences. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 27, 375–389. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Willis, S. L., Tennstedt, S. L., Marsiske, M., Ball, K., Elias, J., Koepke, K. M., Morris, J. N. , ... Wright, E. (2006). Long-term effects of cognitive training on everyday functional outcomes in older adults. Journal of the American Medical Association, 296, 2805–2814. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yang, L., Krampe, R. T. (2009). Long-term maintenance of retest learning in young old and oldest old adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64B, 608–611. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yang, L., Krampe, R. T., Baltes, P. B. (2006). Basic forms of cognitive plasticity extended into the oldest-old: Retest learning, age, and cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, 21, 372–378. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yang, L., Reed, M., Russo, F. A., Wilkinson, A. (2009). A new look at retest learning in older adults: Learning in the absence of item-specific effects. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 64B, 470–473. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar