Abstract
Zusammenfassung. Die Qualität frühkindlicher häuslicher Lernumwelt, insbesondere mütterlichen Interaktionsverhaltens, ist ein bedeutsamer Prädiktor kindlicher Entwicklung. Die Frage, ob diese Interaktionsqualität durch ein schwieriges Temperament des Kindes reduziert ist, wenn kumulierte Belastungsfaktoren ihre Bewältigungskapazitäten einschränken, wurde an 2190 Fällen der Startkohorte 1 des Nationalen Bildungspanels (NEPS) untersucht. Im häuslichen Kontext wurde die Interaktionsqualität über Videoaufnahmen halb-strukturierter Spielsituationen, die übrigen Variablen über computerunterstützte Elterninterviews erhoben, als die Kinder 6 – 8 Monate alt waren. Während in der Nichtrisikogruppe kein Einfluss auf die Interaktionsqualität festzustellen war, zeigte sich in der Risikogruppe (Kumulation von mindestens drei Belastungsfaktoren) ein deutlicher negativer Zusammenhang mit der Neigung des Kindes zu negativen Affektäußerungen, nicht jedoch mit dessen Regulierbarkeit. Negative Affektivität stellt daher besonders in ohnehin gefährdeten Gruppen ein Entwicklungsrisiko dar.
Abstract. The quality of the early home learning environment, especially of maternal interaction behavior, strongly influences child development. Our study investigated whether this quality is reduced by a difficult child temperament in the presence of the cumulated strains of maternal coping capacities. We draw on 2,190 cases from Starting Cohort 1 of the German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS) when children were 6 – 8 months old. The quality of parent–child interactions was assessed in the household context by video coding of semistructured play situations, all other variables were examined via computer-assisted interviews. While there was no relationship in the nonrisk sample, in the risk sample (with at least three cumulated strains) children’s negative affectivity but not their regulatory capacities clearly reduced the interaction quality. This suggests that a difficult temperament poses an additional developmental risk for children growing up under demanding circumstances.
Literatur
1974).
(Infant-mother attachment and social development: ‘Socialisation’ as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals . In M. P. RichardsEd., The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 99 – 135). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.2010). Prevalence of smoking during pregnancy and associated risk factors among Canadian women: A national survey. BMC Pregnancy and Childbirth, 10 (24), 1 – 9.
(2015). Sampling and weighting the sample of the early childhood cohort of the National Educational Panel Study (Technical Report of SUF SC1 Version 2.0.0). Retrieved from https://www.neps-data.de/Portals/0/NEPS/Datenzentrum/Forschungsdaten/SC1/2-0-0/SC1-2-0-0_Weighting.pdf
(2015). The effect of preterm birth on infant negative affect and maternal postpartum depressive symptoms: A preliminary examination in an underrepresented minority sample. Infant Behavior & Development, 39, 159 – 165.
(2015). Temperament bei Kleinkindern: Theoretischer Hintergrund, Operationalisierung im Nationalen Bildungspanel (NEPS) und empirische Befunde aus dem Forschungsprojekt (NEPS Working Paper Nr. 58). Bamberg: Leibniz-Institut für Bildungsverläufe, Nationales Bildungspanel. Verfügbar unter https://www.neps-data.de/Portals/0/Working%20Papers/WP_LVIII.pdf
(1995). Scaffolding children’s learning. Vygotsky and early childhood education. Washington, D.C.: National Association for the Education of Young Children.
(2010). Mutter-Kind-Interaktion im Säuglingsalter, Familienumgebung und Entwicklung früher kognitiver und nicht-kognitiver Fähigkeiten: Eine prospektive Studie (Discussion Paper Nr. 10 – 041). Mannheim: Zentrum für Europäische Wirtschaftsforschung.
(2011). Education as a lifelong process – The German National Educational Panel Study (NEPS). Wiesbaden: Springer VS.
(1994). The effect of infant irritability on mother-infant interaction: A growth-curve analysis. Developmental Psychology, 30, 581 – 590.
(2002). Parenting infants. In M. H. BornsteinEd., Handbook of parenting: Children and parenting (1st ed., pp. 3 – 44). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(2010). Parenting knowledge: Experiential and sociodemographic factors in European American mothers of young children. Developmental Psychology, 46, 1677 – 1693.
(1979). Mother-infant interaction as a function of maternal ratings of temperament. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 10 (2), 67 – 76.
(2002).
(Parenting and social networks . In M. H. BornsteinEd., Handbook of parenting: Social conditions and applied parenting (4th ed., pp. 123 – 148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.2006).
(Social class and socialization in families . In J. E. GrusecP. D. HastingsEds., Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 433 – 460). New York, NY: Guilford Press.2003). Toddlers’ self-regulated compliance to mothers, caregivers, and fathers: Implications for theories of socialization. Developmental Psychology, 39, 680 – 692.
(2011). Prenatal depression effects on early development: A review. Infant Behavior & Development, 34 (1), 1 – 14.
(1995). Maternal factors related to parenting practices, developmental expectations, and perceptions of child behavior problems. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156, 431 – 441.
(2018). Early negative affectivity and regulatory capacity in parental report and scientific observation. Manuscript in preparation.
(1985). Relations between teenagers’ age, knowledge, expectations, and matemal behavior. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3 (1), 47 – 55.
(2003). Studying infant temperament via the Revised Infant Behavior Questionnaire. Infant Behavior & Development, 26 (1), 64 – 86.
(2002).
(Parenting children born preterm . In M. H. BornsteinEd., Handbook of parenting: Children and parenting (1st ed., pp. 329 – 354). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.2012). Bindungen: Das Gefüge psychischer Sicherheit. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.
(2012). Links between maternal education and parenting quality during children’s first three years: The moderating role of income and partner status. Dissertation. University of North Carolina, Greensboro.
(2007). Maternal smoking and fetal growth characteristics in different periods of pregnancy: The generation R study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 165, 1207 – 1215.
(2015). Effects of maternal sensitivity on low birth weight children’s academic achievement: A test of differential susceptibility versus diathesis stress. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 56, 693 – 701.
(2001). Why are children born to teen mothers at risk for adverse outcomes in young adulthood? Results from a 20-year longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 377 – 397.
(2003). The role of the shared family context in differential parenting. Developmental Psychology, 39 (1), 99 – 113.
(2008). Economic deprivation, maternal depression, parenting and children’s cognitive and emotional development in early childhood. The British Journal of Sociology, 59, 783 – 806.
(1988). A comparative analysis of the development and structure of educational systems: Methodological foundations and the construction of a comparative educational scale (CASMIN-Working Paper Nr. 12). Mannheim: Institut für Sozialwissenschaften.
(2001). Differential development of infants at risk for psychopathology: The moderating role of early maternal responsivity. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 43, 292 – 300.
(2010). Predictors of maternal sensitivity to infant distress. Parenting, Science and Practice, 10, 219 – 239.
(2007). Contextual risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28 (1), 40 – 55.
(1960). Robust tests for equality of variances. In I. Olkin & H. Hotelling (Eds.), Contributions to Probability and Statistics: Essays in Honor of Harold Hotelling (pp. 278 – 292). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
(2017).
(Bedingungen sensitiver Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen . In H. WadepohlK. MackowiakK. Fröhlich-GildhoffD. WeltzienHrsg., Interaktionsgestaltung in Familie und Kindertagesbetreuung (S. 27 – 53). Wiesbaden: Springer.2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341, 976 – 980.
(2001). Stability and consistency in mothers’ and infants’ interactive styles. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47 (1), 100 – 120.
(2009). Spontaneous preterm birth and small for gestational age infants in women who stop smoking early in pregnancy: Prospective cohort study. BMJ, 338, b1558. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b1558
(2013). Maternal responsiveness, intrusiveness, and negativity during play with infants: Contextual associations and infant cognitive status in a low-income sample. Infant Mental Health Journal, 34 (1), 80 – 92.
(2016). Maternal responsive-didactic caregiving in play interactions with 10-month-olds and cognitive development at 18 months. Infant and Child Development, 25, 296 – 316.
(2004). Predicting maternal reactivity / sensitivity: The role of infant emotionality, maternal depressiveness / anxiety, and social support. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25 (1), 47 – 61.
(2002).
(Adolescent parenthood . In M.H. BornsteinEd., Handbook of parenting: Being and becoming a parent (3rd ed., pp. 173 – 214). Mahwah, NJ, London, UK: Erlbaum.1999). Child Care and Mother-Child Interaction in the First 3 Years of Life. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1399 – 1413.
(2013). Prediction of toddlers’ expressive language from maternal sensitivity and toddlers’ anger expressions: A developmental perspective. Infant Behavior & Development, 36, 650 – 661.
(2011). Psychologie des Spiels. Weinheim: Beltz.
(2013). OECD framework for statistics on the distribution of household income, consumption and wealth. Paris: OECD Publishing.
(2002).
(Intuitive parenting . In M. H. BornsteinEd., Handbook of parenting: Children and parenting (1st ed., pp. 183 – 203). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.2007). Child negative emotionality and parenting from infancy to preschool: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 43, 438 – 453.
(2008). Meaningful differences in maternal smoking behaviour during pregnancy: Implications for infant behavioural vulnerability. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 62, 318 – 324.
(1977). Genotype environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 309 – 322.
(2002). Child temperament and parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Children and parenting (1st ed., pp. 255 – 277). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
(2005). Are maternal smoking and stress during pregnancy related to ADHD symptoms in children? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 246 – 254.
(2011).
(Auswirkungen öffentlicher Kindertagesbetreuung auf Kinder . In S. WittmannT. RauschenbachH. R. LeuHrsg., Kinder in Deutschland: Eine Bilanz empirischer Studien (S. 173 – 180). Weinheim: Juventa.2006).
(Temperament . In N. EisenbergEd., Handbook of child psychology: Social, Emotional, and Personality Development (ed. 3, pp. 99 – 166). New York, NY: Wiley.2008). Who smokes during pregnancy? An analysis of the German perinatal quality survey 2005. Public Health, 122, 1210 – 1216.
(2007). The association between prenatal exposure to cigarettes and infant and maternal negative affect. Infant Behavior & Development, 30, 387 – 398.
(1965). An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika, 52, 591 – 611.
(2006). The role of early maternal responsiveness in supporting school-aged cognitive development for children who vary in birth status. Pediatrics, 117, 1608 – 1617.
(2011). Mother and father adjustment during early parenthood: The roles of infant temperament and coparenting relationship quality. Infant Behavior & Development, 34, 504 – 514.
(2015). Qualität elterlichen Interaktionsverhaltens: Erfassung von Interaktionen mithilfe der Eltern-Kind-Interaktions Einschätzskala im Nationalen Bildungspanel (NEPS Working Paper Nr. 56). Bamberg: Leibniz-Institut für Bildungsverläufe, Nationales Bildungspanel. Verfügbar unter https://www.neps-data.de/Portals/0/Working%20Papers/WP_LVI.pdf
(1994). Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 425 – 437.
(2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748 – 767.
(2015). The impact of maternal characteristics, infant temperament and contextual factors on maternal responsiveness to infant. Infant Behavior & Development, 40, 1 – 11.
(1977). Temperament and development. New York, NY: Brunner / Mazel.
(2011). Sex differences in parental reaction to pediatric illness. Journal of Child Health Care, 15 (2), 118 – 125.
(2008). The relations among cumulative risk, parenting, and behavior problems during early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 1211 – 1219.
(1949). Comparing individual means in the analysis of variance. Biometrics, 5 (2), 99 – 114.
(2016). Analyzing early child development, influential conditions, and future impacts: Prospects of a German newborn cohort study. International Journal of Child Care and Education Policy, 10, 1 – 20.
(1947). The generalization of ʻstudent’sʼ problem when several different population variances are involved. Biometrika, 34, 28 – 35. https://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/34.1-2.28
(1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571 – 591.
(2016). Aufwachsen unter familiärer Belastung in Deutschland. Bundesgesundheitsblatt, 59, 1262 – 1270.
(2016). Frühe Hilfen und kindliche kognitive Entwicklung: Eine längsschnittliche Pilotuntersuchung psychosozial belasteter Mutter-Kind-Paare in der frühen Kindheit. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 65, 340 – 353.
(