Skip to main content
Original Articles

Implicit Relational Memory in Young and Older Adults

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.25

Abstract. Older adults do not perform as well as young adults in explicit episodic memory tasks that require the formation and retrieval of new associations. Relatively few studies have investigated the effects of older adults' associative deficit on implicit-memory performance. After introducing the reader to the area of implicit-memory research at large, the authors review studies that have investigated young and older adults' performance in implicit associative memory tasks. Core theoretical issues and methodological challenges are discussed.

References

  • Anderson, N.D. Craik, F.I.M. (2000). Memory in the aging brain. In E.Tulving & F.I.M.Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 411-425). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bayen, U.J. Phelps, M.P. Spaniol, J. (2000). Age-related differences in the use of contextual information in recognition memory: A global matching approach. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 55B, 131– 141 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bowers, J.S. Schacter, D.L. (1990). Implicit memory and test awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 404– 416 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buchner, A. Brandt, M. (2003). Further evidence for systematic reliability differences between explicit and implicit memory tests. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 193– 209 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buchner, A. Wippich, W. (2000). On the reliability of implicit and explicit memory measures. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 227– 259 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buckner, R.L. (2003). Functional-anatomic correlates of control processes in memory. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 3999– 4004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bunge, S.A. Burrows, B. Wagner, A.D. (2004). Prefrontal and hippocampal contributions to visual associative recognition: Interactions between cognitive control and episodic retrieval. Brain and Cognition, 56, 141– 152 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cabeza, R. (2006). Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe contributions to relational memory in young and older adults. In D. Zimmer, A. Mecklinger, & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Binding in human memory: A neurocognitive approach (pp. 595-626). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carlesimo, G.A. (1999). Perceptual and conceptual components of repetition priming in anterograde amnesia. In L.G. Nilsson & H.J. Markowitsch, (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of memory (pp. 213-237). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carlesimo, G.A. Perri, R. Costa, A. Serra, L. Caltagirone, C. (2005). Priming for novel between-word associations in patients with organic amnesia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 566– 573 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cermak, L.S. Bleich, R.P. Blackford, S.P. (1988). Deficits in the implicit retention of new associations by alcoholic Korsakoff patients. Brain and Cognition, 7, 312– 323 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chalfonte, B.L. Johnson, M.K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older adults. Memory and Cognition, 24, 403– 416 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cherry, K.E. St. Pierre, C. (1998). Age-related differences in pictorial implicit memory: Role of perceptual and conceptual processes. Experimental Aging Research, 24, 51– 60 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chiarello, C. Hoyer, W.J. (1988). Adult age differences in implicit and explicit memory: Time course and encoding effects. Psychology and Aging, 3, 358– 366 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chun, M.M. Phelps, E.A. (1999). Memory deficits for implicit contextual information in amnesic subjects with hippocampal damage. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 844– 847 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, N.J. Poldrack, R.A. Eichenbaum, H. (1997). Memory for items and memory for relations in the procedural/declarative memory framework. Memory, 5, 131– 178 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coleman, P.D. Flood, D.G. (1987). Neuron numbers and dendritic extent in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiology of Aging, 21, 530– 539 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Craik, F.I.M. Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits: The role of attentional resources. In F.I.M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 191-211). New York: Plenum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davachi, L. Mitchell, J.P. Wagner, A.D. (2003). Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(4), 2157– 2162 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dobbins, I.G. Foley, H. Schacter, D.L. Wagner, A.D. (2002). Executive control during episodic retrieval: Multiple prefrontal processes subserve source memory. Neuron, 35, 989– 996 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eichenbaum, H. Alvarez, P. Ramus, S.J. (2000). Animal models of amnesia. In L.S. Cermak (Ed.), Handbook of neuropsychology: Memory and its disorders (pp. 1-24). Amsterdam: Elsevier First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Erdfelder, E. Bayen, U.J. (1991). Episodisches Gedächtnis im Alter: Methodologische und empirische Argumente für einen Zugang über mathematische Modelle. [Episodic memory in old age: Methodological and empirical arguments for a mathematical modeling approach]. In D. Frey (Ed.), Bericht über den 37. Kongreß der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Kiel 1990 [Proceedings of the 37th Conferences of the German Society for Psychology in Kiel 1990] (Vol. 2, pp. 172-180). Göttingen: Hogrefe First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ergis, A. Van der Linden, M. Deweer, B. (1998). Priming for new associations in normal aging and in mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Cortex, 34, 357– 373 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fleischman, D.A. Gabrieli, J.D.E. (1998). Repetition priming in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease: A review of findings and theories. Psychology and Aging, 13, 88– 119 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fried, I. MacDonald, K.A. Wilson, C.L. (1997). Single neuron activity in human hippocampus and amygdala during recognition of faces and objects. Neuron, 18, 753– 765 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gabrieli, J.D.E. Fleischman, D.A. Keane, M.M. Reminger, S. Morrell, F. (1995). Double dissociation between memory systems underlying explicit and implicit memory in the human brain. Psychological Science, 6, 76– 82 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giovanello, K.S. Schacter, D L. (2006). Reduced hippocampal specificity in aging. Cognitive Neuroscience Society Meeting Abstracts, 232– First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Giovanello, K.S. Schnyer, D.M. Verfaellie, M. (2004). A critical role for the anterior hippocampus in relational memory: Evidence from an fMRI study comparing associative and item recognition. Hippocampus, 14, 5– 8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giovanello, K.S. Verfaellie, M. Keane, M.M. (2003). Disproportionate deficit in associative recognition relative to item recognition in global amnesia. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(13), 186– 194 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gooding, P.A. Mayes, A.R. Van Eijk, R. (1998). A meta-analysis of indirect memory tests for novel material in organic amnesics. Neuropsychologia, 38, 666– 676 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goshen-Gottstein, Y. Moscovitch, M. Melo, B. (2000). Intact implicit memory for newly formed verbal associations in amnesic patients following single study trials. Neuropsychology, 14, 570– 578 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graf, P. Schacter, D.L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 501– 518 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gutchess, A.H. Welsh, R.C. Hedden, T. Bangert, A. Minear, M. Liu, L.L. Park, D.C. (2005). Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: Frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 84– 96 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henke, K. Mondadori, C.R.A. Treyer, V. Nitsch, R.M. Buck, A. Hock, C. (2003a). Nonconscious formation and reactivation of semantic associations by way of the medial temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia, 41, 863– 876 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henke, K. Treyer, V. Nagay, E.T. Kneifel, S. Dursteler, M. Nitsch, R.M. Buck, A. (2003b). Active hippocampus during nonconscious memories. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 31– 48 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Howard, D.V. Fry, A.F. Brune, C.M. (1991). Aging and memory for new associations: Direct versus indirect measures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 779– 792 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Howard, D.V. Heisey, J.G. Shaw, R. (1986). Aging and the priming of newly learned associations. Developmental Psychology, 22, 78– 85 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hoyer, W.J. Verhaeghen, P. (2006). Memory aging. In J. Birren & K.W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (6th ed., pp. 209-232). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L.L. (1983). Remembering the data: Analyzing interactive processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 485– 508 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L.L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513– 541 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacoby, L.L. Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 306– 340 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Java, R.I. Gardiner, J.M. (1991). Priming and aging: Further evidence of preserved memory function. American Journal of Psychology, 104, 89– 100 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jelicic, M. Craik, F.I.M. Moscovitch, M. (1996). Effects of aging on different explicit and implicit tests. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 225– 234 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kinoshita, S. (1999). Priming for novel associations: Evidence for an attentional component. Memory, 7, 385– 404 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Light, L.L. Kennision, R. Prull, M.W. La Voie, D. Zuellig, A. (1996). One-trial associative priming of nonwords in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 11, 417– 430 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Light, L.L. La Voie, D. Valencia-Laver, D. Albertson Owens, S.A. Mead, G. (1992). Direct and indirect measures of memory for modality in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1284– 1297 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Light, L.L. Prull, M.P. La Voie, D.J. Healy, M.R. (2000). Dual-process theories of memory in old age. In T.J. Perfect & E.A. Maylor (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging (pp. 238-300). New York: Oxford University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Light, L.L. Singh, A. (1987). Implicit and explicit memory in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 531– 541 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Light, L. Singh, A. Capps, J. (1986). Dissociation of memory and awareness in young and older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 62– 74 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Monti, L.A. Gabrieli, J.D.E. Reminger, S.L. Rinaldi, J.A. Wilson, R.S. Fleischman, D.A. (1996). Differential effects of aging and Alzheimer's disease on conceptual implicit and explicit memory. Neuropsychology, 10, 101– 112 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moscovitch, M. (1992). Memory and working-with-memory: A component process model based on modules central systems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 257– 267 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Murnane, K. Phelps, M.P. Malmberg, K. (1999). Context-dependent recognition memory: The ICE theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 403– 415 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Musen, G. O'Neill, J.E. (1997). Implicit memory for nonverbal associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 1192– 1202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2000). Adult age differences in memory performance: Tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 1170– 1187 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Naveh-Benjamin, M. Guez, J. Kilb, A. Reedy, S. (2004). The associative memory deficit of older adults: Further support using face-name associations. Psychology and Aging, 19, 541– 546 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Naveh-Benjamin, M. Hussain, Z. Guez, J. Bar-On, M. (2003). Adult age differences in episodic memory: Further support for an associative-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29, 826– 837 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • O'Hanlon, L. Wilcox, K.A. Kemper, S. (2001). Age differences in implicit and explicit associative memory: Exploring elaborative processing effects. Experimental Aging Research, 27, 341– 359 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Park, H. Quinlan, J. Thornton, E. Reder, L.M. (2004). The effect of midazolam on visual search: Implications for understanding amnesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 17879– 17883 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Poldrack, R.A. Cohen, N.J. (1997). Priming of new associations in reading time: What is learned?. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, 398– 402 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prince, S.E. Daselaar, S.M. Cabeza, R. (2005). Neural correlates of relational memory: Successful encoding and retrieval of semantic and perceptual associations. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 1203– 1210 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reingold, E.M. Goshen-Gottstein, Y. (1996a). Automatic retrieval of new associations under shallow encoding conditions. Consciousness and Cognition, 5, 117– 130 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reingold, E.M. Goshen-Gottstein, Y. (1996b). Separating consciously controlled and automatic influences in memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 397– 406 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riefer, D.M. Batchelder, W.H. (1991). Age differences in storage and retrieval: A multinomial modeling analysis. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 415– 418 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roediger, H.L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 1043– 1056 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roediger, H.L. Blaxton, T.A. (1987). Effects of varying modality, surface features, and retention interval on priming in word fragment completion. Memory and Cognition, 15, 379– 388 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ryan, J.D. Althoff, R.R. Whitlow, S. Cohen, N.J. (2000). Amnesia is a deficit in relational memory. Psychological Science, 11, 454– 461 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salthouse, T.A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403– 428 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Savage, G.R. Saling, M.M. Davis, C.W. Berkovic, S. (2002). Direct and indirect measures of verbal relational memory following anterior temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia, 40, 302– 316 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schacter, D.L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501– 518 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schacter, D.L. Graf, P. (1989). Modality specificity of implicit memory for new associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 3– 12 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schacter, D.L. Wagner, A.D. Buckner, R.L. (2000). Memory systems of 1999. In E. Tulving & F.I.M. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 627-643). New York: Oxford University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shimamura, A.P. (1986). Priming effects in amnesia: Evidence for a dissociable memory function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 38, 619– 644 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, A.D. Hultsch, D.F. Masson, M.E.J. (1995). Adult age differences in perceptually based, but not conceptually based implicit tests of memory. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, 162– 170 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Velanova, K. Jacoby, L.L. Wheeler, M.E. McAvoy, M.P. Peterson, S.E. Buckner, R.L. (2003). Functional-anatomic correlates of sustained and transient processing components engaged during controlled retrieval. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 8460– 8470 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Warrington, E.K. Weizkrantz, L. (1970). Amnesic syndrome: Consolidation or retrieval. Nature, 274, 972– 974 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zacks, R.T. Hasher, L. Li, K.Z. (2000). Human memory. In F.I.M. Craik & T.a. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 293-357). New Jersey: Erlbaum First citation in articleGoogle Scholar