Skip to main content
Open AccessResearch Trends

The Role of Bystanders in the Prevention of Railway Suicides in New South Wales, Australia

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0227-5910/a000804

Abstract

Abstract.Background: Bystanders can play a key role in preventing railway suicides by taking direct action or by raising an alarm. Aims: The study investigated in this context: (1) the prevalence of preventative actions by bystanders; (2) the relationship, if any, between first-hand preventions by bystanders and the degree of ambiguity around the imminence of danger; and (3) the nature of first-hand preventions by bystanders. Method: Data were obtained from a security reporting database at Sydney Trains for 2011–2019 for accident and suicide-related incidents (N = 1,278). Results: In 635 cases of suicide prevention, bystanders were identified as first responders in 11% of cases and as raising the alarm in 11% of cases. Bystanders as first responders intervened proportionally more where the ambiguity of danger is low (jumping) compared with high (sitting, standing, wandering). Of the 69 cases of bystander preventions, 77% involved physical interaction (e.g., holding back) and 49% involved more than one bystander. Limitations: The data source could be biased because of incompleteness or nonstandard reporting. Conclusion: Rail policy-makers should consider education and support for bystanders and staff: for example, by making known the prevalence of helping, the importance of intervening, and what types of intervention are most helpful.

The problem of railway suicides and attempted suicides is an important one. Apart from the personal impact on the individuals involved, there are significant consequences for railway staff, eyewitnesses, other passengers, and rail network operators. Bystanders can play a key role in cases of suspected suicide attempts on railway networks. Such cases can occur at times or locations where bystanders are the only individuals who are able to react by taking direct action themselves or by raising the alarm so others can intervene. The downside of bystander interventions is the person intervening may place themselves at risk of injury and suffer subsequent trauma (Liebst et al., 2018).

Direct interventions by bystanders are defined as actions that bystanders take first-hand to prevent a suicide attempt (Berkowitz, 2009). Examples in the railway context include actions such as entering the track to physically remove a person from the path of a train, stopping a person from jumping from the platform into the path of an oncoming train, or engaging in a conversation with an individual perceived as at risk. These actions are in contrast to indirect intervention, in which bystanders intervene by contacting emergency services or relevant authorities. While there have been field studies into bystander interventions in contexts including crime (Huston et al., 1981), medical emergencies (Faul et al., 2016), surf lifesaving (Attard et al., 2015), and suicide in a public setting (Owens et al., 2019), there have been few dealing with such interventions in the context of railway networks.

Even when a suicide attempt is suspected, there is a chance that bystanders will not react until it is too late, or will not react at all. The bystander effect has been posited – a reluctance of individuals to offer help to someone at risk when other people are present. It is thought that the greater the number of bystanders, the less likely one of them will help in a critical situation (Darley & Latane, 1968). Latanė and Darley (1970) proposed a five-stage model of bystander intervention: noticing; interpreting as an emergency; developing a feeling of personal responsibility; a belief that one has the skills to help; and reaching a conscious decision to help. Other research has found bystanders may be more likely to engage in helping behaviors if they are confronted with dangerous emergencies, where the bystander effect may be attenuated. Three explanations of this phenomenon are advanced by Fischer et al. (2011). First, dangerous situations are more likely to be construed as having low situational ambiguity, which increases experience arousal, and thus helping responses. Second, in these cases, other bystanders can be seen as providers of physical support, and thus reduce fear of intervention by one individual. Third, some dangerous emergencies can only be effectively resolved by cooperation and coordination among several bystanders. A large recent study using closed-circuit television (CCTV) footage of real-life bystander intervention in public contexts showed that in 9 out of 10 cases at least one bystander, and typically several, would intervene to help (Philpot et al., 2019).

Prevention of railway suicides is often considered in terms of the behavior by the individual at risk immediately prior to an incident. The nature and opportunity for successful interventions can depend on the nature of this pre-crash behavior. Dinkel et al. (2011) distinguished three broad groupings of pre-crash behavior: jumping, lying, and wandering. Passive measures such as physical barriers and fencing have been shown to reduce the relative incidence of lying and wandering attempts compared with jumping incidents (Gregor et al., 2019). Active interventions may be fewer in cases of jumping, where an individual makes an attempt by jumping or leaping into the path of a train, compared with cases where an individual is stationary or wandering on tracks for a period, allowing more time for an intervention. On the other hand, an attempt at self-harm by jumping into the path of a train has less situational ambiguity and the imminence of danger is more evident to observers.

The sparseness of research in the area of bystander interventions to prevent suicides on railway networks indicates further work is required. Thus, the aims of the current study were to investigate in the context of railway suicide attempts: (1) the prevalence of preventative actions by bystanders; (2) the relationship, if any, between active, first-hand preventions by bystanders and the degree of ambiguity around the imminence of danger; and (3) the nature of first-hand preventions by bystanders. In terms of an integrated systems approach to suicide prevention (Black Dog Institute, 2019), the study contributes by informing potential strategies for gatekeeper training, reducing access to means, and community awareness programs.

Method

Our research setting was two heavy rail networks in the state of New South Wales (NSW) in Australia: the Sydney metropolitan network and the regional NSW network. Sydney Trains safety reports show a number of initiatives that can be classified as suicide reduction interventions (e.g., Sydney Trains, 2018). Sydney Trains’ policy regarding responsible media reporting of suicides and attempted suicides has been practiced over the whole of the study period. Fencing has been demonstrated to have a significant effect on preventing railway suicide, comparing the relatively well-fenced Sydney metropolitan network with regional NSW’s relatively unfenced network (Gregor et al., 2019). The Security Control Centre, acting across NSW, coordinates communication and responses by station staff, police, ambulance, and other stakeholders in cases of emergency, in accordance with standard operating procedures and using mobile communication and CCTV.

The primary data source was a security reporting database maintained for the years 2011–2019 for incidents across the whole NSW rail network. The database is maintained for internal operational purposes by Sydney Trains Security in NSW (see Beavan, 2016). Data are entered during and after each incident by response operators. Fields are maintained for time, day of week, month, year, location, region, and outcome, among others. Outcomes are distinguished as apparent suicide, attempted suicide, death by apparent misadventure, and death (suspicious/undetermined). There is a textual free-form incident summary of 4,192 characters from which further data fields were obtained, where possible, for attempted suicides, including gender, pre-crash behaviors, and relevant variables regarding characteristics of the situation (location, availability of staff, train movement, access points, interveners, etc.). Coding was done manually. For details on the coding, see Electronic Supplementary Material 1 (ESM 1). Due to the nature of the data, nonparametric statistical tests were used (χ2 tests). A significance value of p < .05 was used, unless otherwise noted.

Results

There has been some degree of success in combatting rail suicide in NSW. The incidence of train suicides remained relatively constant over the period 2011–2019, with an average of 0.28 cases annually per 100,000 head of population in NSW. The incidence of suicides overall in NSW increased significantly from 8.5 in 2011 to 11.22 in 2018 per 100,000 (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2019a, 2019b). The relatively low rate of rail suicide is in part attributed to the high level and standard of fencing in the Sydney metropolitan area, which deters some attempts (Gregor et al., 2019).

Other factors, however, also play a part. The Sydney Trains database shows there was a high incidence of “prevented suicides,” where there was clear evidence that an individual was attempting to come in contact with a train for the purpose of self-harm but was prevented from injury or death by the actions of others. Although the rate of rail suicide has remained relatively constant, the rate of attempted suicides has increased markedly, even with the high level of fencing. For the period 2011–2019, the database contained 1,278 cases in total, including cases of misadventure (accident). Of these cases, 984 cases were classed as “attempted suicide” (including non-train attempts). There were 635 cases of attempts at suicide by train that were prevented. There were also 106 injuries and 191 deaths by suicide involving trains.

For the current study, our interest was in the actors who take a role in the active prevention of train suicides (635 cases). Figure 1 shows the frequency of those who were involved in preventions, in terms of “first responders” to an attempt; namely, bystanders, rail personnel at stations, other rail personnel, emergency services, and other/unknown.

Figure 1 Frequencies for active preventions by first-responder category (n = 635).

The incidence of bystanders known to be first responders is reasonably high, at 69 of the 635 cases (11%). Bystanders also raised the alarm and reported, but were not first responders, in another 70 cases. Thus, the total number of cases where bystanders either were first responders or raised the alarm is 139, 22% of the 635 preventions. If bystanders had not played a role in suicide preventions, the number of deaths by suicide could have been 139 cases (73%) higher than the actual number of 191.

The literature on bystanders’ roles in helping others suggests that bystanders are more likely to provide aid when there is less ambiguity around the danger that the individual at risk faces. In the rail suicide context, the situation in which there is likely to be less ambiguity is when an individual jumps from a platform into the path of a train. The incidents in our database allowed us to distinguish between three categories for pre-crash behavior: (1) jumping; (2) lying, sitting, or standing on tracks; and (3) wandering or running on tracks. In subsequent analysis we distinguish between the first category of jumping, and “non-jumping” (the other two categories combined), and consider the jumping category as being more unambiguously dangerous. It is expected that bystander preventions as first responders will occur comparatively more often for cases in the jumping category.

Table 1 shows results for the first-responder categories in the cases of prevention. The proportion of prevented jumping cases for the bystander category (36%) is higher than in other categories (excluding the unknown/other category). The next highest is for rail personnel on stations (26%). Analysis of cases where the responder and pre-crash category could be determined (the bolded figures) shows there is a significant difference among the four main responder categories, in terms of the proportion of preventions for jumping versus non-jumping cases, χ2(3, N = 384) = 26.1, p < .001.

Table 1 Analysis of pre-crash behavior vs. first-responder category

There may be factors other than lack of ambiguity that influence the readiness of bystanders to intervene. It may be also because they are more ready-to-hand, or in fact that they are the only people close by, but it does indicate their willingness to step up when “danger is imminent.” Table E3 in ESM 1 summarizes the differences between bystander preventions and other preventions on a range of factors that could be ascertained from the database and could have some bearing on a bystander’s preparedness to take action, with results of tests of significance. Comparable figures for deaths by suicide are also presented as a point of reference. Analyses are shown separately for metropolitan Sydney and regional NSW because of the differences between these regions in terms of pre-crash behavior reported by Gregor et al. (2019).

This analysis shows the only significant differences between bystander and non-bystanders are for pre-crash behavior and location (see Table 2). These two factors are related, since nearly all prevented jumping incidents occur at platforms or tracks between platforms (90.6%), while prevented non-jumping incidents occur at a lower percentage at platforms (59.4%) and more incidents on tracks between stations. No significant differences were found for gender. That is, bystanders were not more likely to prevent attempts by females than by males, or the reverse. This situation differs from cases of death by suicide in our study, where males are more likely than females to die by suicide. This imbalance has been observed elsewhere (Freeman et al., 2017).

Table 2 Comparison of bystander active preventions, other active preventions, and deaths by suicide

The 69 cases of bystander preventions were analyzed in further detail to provide insights into how they were enacted. Table 3 provides excerpts from the database showing how bystanders acted in different circumstances, when the person at risk made a jumping attempt, when they were non-jumping, at a station or not, and the different types of actions that took place. Case 1 shows an example where the person had been wandering about for a period but action was only taken when he attempted to jump, supporting the idea that a bystander is more likely to intervene when the situation is less ambiguously dangerous.

Table 3 Examples showing details of active preventions by bystanders (n = 69)

A number of the cases, as Table 3 illustrates, involved physical interaction between the responders and the person at risk. In fact, physical interaction occurred in 85% of the 69 active prevention cases. Minimal or light interaction, such as blocking or guiding, occurred in 9% of these cases. Heavier interactions, such as tackling, pulling, holding, grabbing, or restraining, occurred in 77%. In 48% of these cases, the bystander acted alone, at least in the first instance, and in 49% of cases more than one bystander acted together (the number was unclear in 3%).

Discussion

The study’s first research theme concerns the extent to which bystanders assisted in the prevention of rail suicides. The incidence of bystanders as first responders who actively prevented suicides was 11% (69 of 635). Close to 22% of all preventions in the NSW railway network in the period 2011–2019 had identified bystander involvement, including bystanders as either reporters of incidents or as first responders (139 of 635). This figure compares favorably with one of the few comparable studies to be found, where data from the United Kingdom suggest that approximately 1 in 10 interventions of all types – direct and indirect – on the railway are by the public (Samaritans, 2018).

The second theme concerns the relationship between bystander interventions and situational ambiguity. There was a significant difference for the situations in which bystanders responded in terms of pre-crash behaviors – jumping versus non-jumping. Known jumping incidents were approximately 15.1% of the total of 635 prevented incidents. Bystanders contributed more than would be expected in this category, as 27.5% of the 69 bystander preventions were known jumping incidents, compared with 13.6% known jumping incidents of 566 preventions by other parties. This finding is congruent with other work showing that bystanders were more likely to help in situations where there was low ambiguity about the risk of danger (e.g., Fischer et al., 2011). Interpretation of this finding in terms of the classic bystander intervention model of Latanė and Darley (1970) suggests that for jumping cases, the stages of noticing, interpreting, and knowing what to do may be more readily accomplished. For more ambiguous cases, such as wandering, education could assist bystanders in noticing, interpreting, and knowing how to help.

The study’s third theme concerns the nature of bystander preventions. No significant differences were found between bystander and non-bystander preventions in terms of the gender of the person at risk or the time of day. Bystander preventions were proportionally more likely to occur at or near platforms, compared with open tracks, which is not surprising as bystanders are more likely to be present at platforms. A surprising proportion of the incidents in which bystanders intervened shows a high degree of physicality in interactions, with bystanders tackling, grabbing, or holding the person at risk. In almost half the cases of bystander preventions, there was more than one bystander involved.

Limitations

The study has certain limitations. Some factors of potential interest, including intoxication, age, ethnicity, occupation, and economic status, are not routinely reported. The values of some factors could not be determined for all cases and had to be treated as unknown. Further, it is not always possible to distinguish cases of apparent suicide attempts from those where other forms of self-harm are intended.

Conclusion

The railway literature has largely overlooked the role and effectiveness of the public as first responders and reporters in ensuring a safe railway system. Our study shows that over the period of 2011–2019, the number of deaths by suicide could have been up to 73% higher without bystander interventions. Theoretically, our study provides support for Fischer et al. (2011) findings regarding bystander intervention in a clear-cut emergency, where a bystander is more likely to intervene when there is less ambiguity around the risk of danger.

The study also has important practical implications. Efforts to ensure data collected in organizations are as complete and accurate as possible are recommended, across jurisdictions, so that further studies can be undertaken. Here and more generally, the relatively large number of incidents of bystander preventions has implication for policy in public educational campaigns and support for those brave bystanders who risk their own safety. The known interventions by rail staff at 16% is not much higher than the known interventions by bystanders at 11%. Rail operators should continue their efforts to educate staff in recognizing potential danger and knowing how to help. Further, there are a proportion of cases of deaths by suicide that occur away from platforms (n = 50, 26%), where bystanders and staff are less likely to be able to help. Further work on the efficacy of signage such as Lifeline billboards and preventative barriers is suggested.

Electronic Supplementary Material

The electronic supplementary material is available with the online version of the article at https://doi.org/10.1027/0227-5910/a000804

Author Biographies

Nguyen Viet Ngo, MIntMgt, is a research associate at the Research School of Management at the Australian National University, Canberra, Australia, working as a consultant for the Sydney Trains Project. His primary fields of research are management and international business.

Shirley Gregor, PhD, is Emeritus Professor at the Australian National University, Canberra, Australia. Her primary field of research is information systems and she had led large projects and published in areas including artificial intelligence, human–computer interaction, and the adoption and use of transformative information technologies.

Gary Beavan is the Director Security for Sydney Trains and has held senior leadership roles in the rail industry, intelligence, and law enforcement. He has led programs that improved customer journey times and the security of public transport. Gary Beavan has partnered the rail industry, health professionals, and academics to champion awareness and understanding of suicide.

Bianca Riley, MBA, is an outcomes-focused professional with over 12 years of experience in the rail industry. She has a problem-solving mindset and is not afraid to challenge the norm in her drive for continuous improvement. Bianca Riley also brings first-hand experience in suicide prevention in the rail environment.

References