Skip to main content
Articles

Selective Attention and Error Processing in an Illusory Conjunction Task

An Event-Related Brain Potential Study

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.19.3.216

Abstract. We recorded event-related potentials in an illusory conjunction task, in which subjects were cued on each trial to search for a particular colored letter in a subsequently presented test array, consisting of three different letters in three different colors. In a proportion of trials the target letter was present and in other trials none of the relevant features were present. In still other trials one of the features (color or letter identity) were present or both features were present but not combined in the same display element. When relevant features were present this resulted in an early posterior selection negativity (SN) and a frontal selection positivity (FSP). When a target was presented, this resulted in a FSP that was enhanced after 250 ms as compared to when both relevant features were present but not combined in the same display element. This suggests that this effect reflects an extra process of attending to both features bound to the same object. There were no differences between the ERPs in feature error and conjunction error trials, contrary to the idea that these two types of errors are due to different (perceptual and attentional) mechanisms. The P300 in conjunction error trials was much reduced relative to the P300 in correct target detection trials. A similar, error-related negativity-like component was visible in the response-locked averages in correct target detection trials, in feature error trials, and in conjunction error trials. Dipole modeling of this component resulted in a source in a deep medial-frontal location. These results suggested that this type of task induces a high level of response conflict, in which decision-related processes may play a major role.

References

  • Alain, C. , McNeely, H.E. , He, Y. , Christensen, B.K. , West, R. (2002). Neurophysiological evidence of error-monitoring deficits in patients with schizophrenia. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 840– 846 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ashby, F.G. , Prinzmetal, W. , Ivry, R. , Maddox, W.T. (1996). A formal theory of feature binding in object perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 60– 100 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Braver, T.S. , Barch, D.M. , Gray, J.R. , Molfese, D.L. , Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects of frequency, inhibition, and errors. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 825– 836 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, C.S. , Braver, T.S. , Barch, S.D.M. , Botvinick, M.M. , Noll, D. , Cohen, J.D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 280, 747– 749 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coles, M.G.H. , Scheffers, M.K. , Holroyd, C.B. (2001). Why is there an ERN/NE on correct trials? Response representations, stimulus-related components, and the theory of error-processing. Biological Psychology, 56, 173– 189 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Craver-Lemley, C. , Arterberry, M.E. , Reeves, A. (1999). “Illusory” conjunctions: The conjoining of features of visual and imagined stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1036– 1049 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dehaene, S. , Posner, M.I. , Tucker, D.M. (1994). Localization of a neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 5, 303– 305 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Donk, M. (1999). Illusory conjunctions are an illusion: The effects of target-nontarget similarity on conjunction and feature errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1207– 1233 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Driver, J. , Baylis, C. (1998). Attention and visual object segmentation. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp.299-325). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eimer, M. (1995). Event-related potential correlates of transient attention shifts to color and location. Biological Psychology, 41, 167– 182 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hohnsbein, J. , Hoormann, J. , Blanke, L. (1990). Effects of errors in choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided attention. In C.H.M. Brunia, A.W.K. Gaillard, & A. Kok (Eds.), Psychophysiological brain research (pp.192-195). Tilburg: Tilburg University Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hohnsbein, J. , Hoormann, J. , Blanke, L. (1991). Effects of crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components: II. Error processing in choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78, 447– 455 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Falkenstein, M. , Hoormann, J. , Christ, S. , Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction time errors and their functional significance: A tutorial. Biological Psychology, 52, 87– 107 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gehring, W.J. , Coles, M.G. , Meyer, D.E. , Donchin, E. (1995). A brain potential manifestation of error-related processing. In G. Karmos, M. Molnar, V. Csepe, I. Czigler, & J.E. Desmedt (Eds.), Perspectives of event-related potentials research (EEG Journal Supplement 44, pp.261-272). Amsterdam: Elsevier First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gehring, W.J. , Goss, B. , Coles, M.G. , Meyer, D.E. , Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385– 390 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gehring, W.J. , Willoughby, A.R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295, 2279– 2282 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hansen, J.C. , Hillyard, S.A. (1983). Selective attention to multidimensional auditory stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 1– 19 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harter, M.R. , Anllo-Vento, L. (1991). Visual-spatial attention: Preparation and selection in children and adults. In C.H.M. Brunia, G. Mulder, G., & M.N. Verbaten (Eds.), Event-related potentials of the brain (pp.1832-94). Amsterdam: Elsevier First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hillyard, S.A. , Hansen, J.C. (1986). Attention: Electrophysiological approaches. In M.G.H. Coles, E. Donchin, & S.W. Porges (Eds.), Psychophysiology: Systems, processes, and applications (pp.227-243). New York: Guilford First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hillyard, S.A. , Münte, T.F. (1984). Selective attention to color and location: An analysis with event-related brain potential. Perception and Psychophysics, 36, 185– 198 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ivry, R.B. , Prinzmetal, W. (1991). Effect of feature similarity on illusory conjunctions. Perception and Psychophysics, 49, 105– 116 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kenemans, J.L. , Kok, A. , Smulders, F.T.Y. (1993). Event-related potentials to conjunctions of spatial frequency and orientation as a function of stimulus parameters and response requirements. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 88, 51– 63 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kenemans, J.L. , Lijfijt, M. , Camfferman, G. , Verbaten, M.N. (2002). Split-second sequential selective activation in human secondary visual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 48– 61 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kenemans, J.L. , Smulders, F.T.Y. , Kok, A. (1995). Selective processing of two-dimensional visual stimuli in young and old subjects: Electrophysiological analysis. Psychophysiology, 32, 108– 120 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Khurana, B. (1988). Visual structure and the integration of form and color information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1766– 1785 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kiehl, K.A. , Liddle, P.F. , Hopfinger, J.B. (2000). Error processing and the rostral anterior cingulate: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology, 37, 216– 223 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lange, J.J. , Wijers, A.A. , Mulder, L.J.M. , Mulder, G. (1999). ERP effects of spatial attention and display search with unilateral and bilateral stimulus displays. Biological Psychology, 50, 203– 233 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luck, S.J. , Girelli, M. (1998). Electrophysiological approaches to the study of selective attention in the human brain. In R. Parasuraman (Ed.), The attentive brain (pp.71-94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Luck, S.J. , Hillyard, S.A. , Mouloua, M. , Woldorff, M.G. , Clark, V.P. , Hawkins, H.L. (1994). Effects of spatial cuing on luminance detectability: Psychophysical and electrophysiological evidence for early selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 887– 904 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luu, P. , Flaisch, T. , Tucker, D.M. (2000). Medial frontal cortex in action monitoring. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 464– 469 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mangun, G.R. , Hillyard, S.A. (1991). Modulation of sensory-evoked brain potentials provide evidence for changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 1057– 1074 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mathalon, D.H. , Fedor, M. , Faustman, W.O. , Gray, M. , Asakari, N. , Ford, M.J. (2002). Response-monitoring dysfunction in schizophrenia: An event-related brain potential study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 22– 41 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Navon, D. , Ehrlich, B. (1995). Illusory conjunctions: Does inattention really matter?. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 59– 83 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nobre, A.C. , Sebestyen, G.N. , Miniussi, C. (2000). The dynamics of shifting attention revealed by event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 38, 964– 974 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perrin, F. , Pernier, J. , Bertrand, O. , Echalier, J.F. (1989). Spherical splines for scalp potential and current density mapping. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 72, 184– 187 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Posner, M.I. , Snyder, C.R.R. , Davidson, B.J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160– 174 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Previc, F.H. , Harter, M.R. (1982). Electrophysiological and behavioral indicants of selective attention to multifeature gratings. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 465– 472 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prinzmetal, W. , Henderson, D. , Ivry, R. (1995). Loosening the constraints on illusory conjunctions: Assessing the roles of exposure duration and attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1362– 1375 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prinzmetal, W. , Hoffman, H. , Vest, K. (1991). Automatic processes in word perception: An analysis from illusory conjunctions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 902– 923 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rugg, M.D. , Milner, A.D. , Lines, C.R. , Phalp, R. (1987). Modulation of visual event-related potentials by spatial and nonspatial visual selective attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 85– 96 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scherg, M. (1990). Fundamentals of dipole source potential analysis. In F. Grandori, M. Hoke, & G.L. Romani (Eds.),Auditory evoked magnetic fields and electric potentials. Advances in audiology, 6 (pp.40-69). Basel: Karger First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smid, H.G.O.M. , Jakob, A. , Heinze, H.J. (1999). An event-related brain potential study of visual selective attention to conjunctions of color and shape. Psychophysiology, 36, 264– 279 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sutton, S. , Ruchkin, D.S. , Munson, R. , Kietzman, M.L. (1982). Event-related potentials in a two-interval forced-choice detection task. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 360– 374 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Treisman, A.M. , Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97– 136 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Treisman, A.M. , Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95, 15– 48 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Treisman, A.M. , Sato, S. (1990). Conjunction search revisited. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 394– 400 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Treisman, A. , Schmidt, H. (1982). Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 107– 141 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Treisman, A. , Souther, J. (1986). Illusory words: The roles of attention and of top-down constraints in conjoining letters to form words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12, 3– 17 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tsal, Y. (1989). Do illusory conjunctions support the feature integration theory? A critical review of theory and findings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 394– 100 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tsal, Y. , Meiran, N. , Lavie, N. (1994). The role of attention in illusory conjunctions. Perception and Psychophysics, 55, 350– 358 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ungerleider, L.G. , Haxby, J.V. (1994). “What” and “where” in the human brain. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, 4, 157– 165 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Veen, V. , Carter, C.S. (2002a). The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: FMRI and ERP studies. Physiology and Behavior, 77, 477– 482 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Veen, V. , Carter, C.S. (2002b). The timing of action-monitoring processes in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 593– 602 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vidal, F. , Hasbroucq, T. , Grapperon, J. , Bonnet, M. (2000). Is the “error negativity” specific to errors?. Biological Psychology, 51, 109– 128 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wijers, A.A. , Lamain, W. , Slopsema, S. , Mulder, G. , Mulder, L.J.M. (1989). An electrophysiological investigation of the spatial distribution of attention to colored stimuli in focused and divided attention conditions. Biological Psychology, 29, 213– 245 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wijers, A.A. , Mulder, G. , Okita, T. , Mulder, L.J.M. (1989). An ERP-study on memory search and selective attention to lettersize and conjunctions of lettersize and color. Psychophysiology, 26, 529– 547 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wijers, A.A. , Mulder, G. , Okita, T. , Mulder, L.J.M. , Scheffers, M.K. (1989). Attention to colour: An ERP-analysis of selection, controlled search, and motor activation. Psychophysiology, 26, 89– 109 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wijers, A.A. , Van Besouw, N.J.P. , Mulder, G. (2002). Selective attention to a facial feature with and without facial context: An ERP-study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 44, 13– 35 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar