Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.20.3.160

In the present study event-related potentials (ERPs) and event-related lateralizations (ERLs) were analyzed to investigate mechanisms of attentional inhibition engaged when a target stimulus has to be located within a simultaneous target-distractor display. The putative after-effects of inhibition were examined with a prime-probe technique by comparing a “DT” condition (the prime Distractor location becomes the probe Target location) with a control condition (the probe target appears at a previously empty position). The specific aim was to dissociate more “automatic” aspects from more “controlled” aspects associated with the inhibition of distractor locations. To do so, we compared physically identical prime-probe pairs in a sustained-attention context (same target throughout a block) and a transient-attention context (trial-by-trial target specification). Three early ERP/ERL components showed differential effects for DT compared to control: (1) the posterior N1 with a diminished amplitude contralateral to the visual half-field side of target presentation, (2) the N2pc with an enhanced amplitude contralateral to the visual half-field side of target presentation, and (3) the posteriorly distributed N2 with a nonlateralized enhancement for DT compared to control. These effects were differently affected by the context manipulation. While the N2pc effect was observed exclusively under sustained attention, the N1 lateralization effect and the N2 effect were not differentially modulated. The N1 lateralization effect seems consistent with an inhibition-of-return explanation. The N2pc and N2 effects are supposed to be reflecting different aspects of a biased-competition model of distractor inhibition.

References

  • Alho, K. , Lavikainen, J. , Reinikainen, K. , Sams, M. , Näätänen, R. (1990). Event-related brain potentials in selective listening to frequent and rare stimuli Psychophysiology, 27, 73–86. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chelazzi, L. , Duncan, J. , Miller, E.K. , Desimone, R. (1998). Responses of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual search Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 2918–2940. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Christie, J. , Klein, R.M. (2001). Negative priming for spatial location? Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 24–38. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coles, M.G.H. , Gratton, G. , Donchin, E. (1995). Mental chronometry and the study of human information processing. In M.D. Rugg, M.G.H. Coles (Eds.), Electrophysiology of mind: Event-related brain potentials and cognition (pp. 86– 131). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Desimone, R. (1998). Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 353(1373), 1245–1255. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Desimone, R. , Wessinger, M. , Thomas, L. , Schneider, W. (1990). Attentional control of visual perception: Cortical and subcortical mechanisms Cold Spring Harbour Symposium on Quantitative Biology, 55, 963–971. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99, 225–234. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eimer, M. (1997). An event-related potential (ERP) study of transient and sustained visual attention to color and form Biological Psychology, 44, 143–160. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distractors in visual selection: A review Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2, 145–173. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gratton, G. , Coles, M.G.H. , Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 468–484. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hansen, J.C. , Hillyard, S.A. (1988). Temporal dynamics of human auditory selective attention Psychophysiology, 25, 316–329. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heinze, H.J. , Luck, S.J. , Mangun, G.R. , Hillyard, S.A. (1990). Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. I. Evidence for early selection Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, 511–527. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopf, J.M. , Boelmans, K. , Schoenfeld, M.A. , Luck, S.J. , Heinze, H.J. (2004). Attention to features precedes attention to locations in visual search: Evidence from electromagnetic brain responses in humans Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 1822–1832. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopf, J.M. , Luck, S.J. , Girelli, M. , Hagner, T. , Mangun, G.R. , Scheich, H. (2000). Neural sources of focused attention in visual search Cerebral Cortex, 10, 1233–1241. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Houghton, G. , Tipper, S.P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. In D. Dagenbach, T.H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention and language (pp. 53–112). London: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kok, A. (1999). Varieties of inhibition: Manifestations in cognition, event-related potentials, and aging Acta Psychologica, 101, 129–158. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kruschke, J.K. (2003). Attention in learning Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 171–171. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lubow, R.E. , Kaplan, O. (1997). Visual search as a function of type of prior experience with target and distractor Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 14–24. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luck, S.J. , Fan, S. , Hillyard, S.A. (1993). Attention-related modulation of sensory-evoked brain activity in a visual search task Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 188–195. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luck, S.J. , Girelli, M. , McDermott, M.T. , Ford, M.A. (1997). Bridging the gap between monkey neurophysiology and human perception: An ambiguity resolution theory of visual selective attention Cognitive Psychology, 33, 64–87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luck, S.J. , Heinze, H.J. , Mangun, G.R. , Hillyard, S.A. (1990). Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, 528–542. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mackintosh, N.J. (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in associability of stimuli with reinforcement Psychological Review, 82, 276–298. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mangun, G.R. , Hillyard, S.A. (1995). Mechanisms and models of selective attention. In M.D. Rugg, M.G.H. Coles (Eds.), Electrophysiology of mind: Event-related brain potentials and cognition (pp. 40–85). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • May, C.P. , Kane, M.J. , Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming Psychological Bulletin, 118, 35–54. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mayr, S. , Niedeggen, M. , Buchner, A. , Pietrowsky, R. (2003). ERP correlates of auditory negative priming Cognition, 90(2), B11–21. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Milliken, B. , Tipper, S.P. , Houghton, G. , Lupianez, J. (2000). Attending, ignoring, and repetition: On the relation between negative priming and inhibition of return Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 1280–1296. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. (1990). The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed by event-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 201–288. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neill, W.T. , Valdes, L.A. , Terry, K.M. , Gorfein, D.S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 993–1000. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Park, J. , Kanwisher, N. (1994). Negative priming for spatial locations: Identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 613–623. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pratt, J. , Abrams, R.A. (1999). Inhibition of return in discrimination tasks Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 229–242. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prime, D.J. , Ward, L.M. (2004). Inhibition of return from stimulus to response Psychological Science, 15, 272–276. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shiffrin, R.M. , Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory Psychological Review, 84, 127–190. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tipper, S.P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 321–343. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tipper, S.P. , McLaren, J. (1990). Evidence for efficient selectivity in children. In J.T. Enns (Ed.), The development of attention: Research and theory (pp. 197–210). Amsterdam: North-Holland. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tipper, S.P. , Weaver, B. , Cameron, S. , Brehaut, J. , Bastedo, J. (1991). Inhibitory mechanisms of attention in identification and localization tasks: Time course and disruption Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 681–692. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wijers, A.A. , Mulder, G. , Gunter, T.C. , Smid, H.G.O.M. (1996). Die Hirnelektrische Analyse der selektiven Aufmerksamkeit [Brain-electrical analysis of selective attention]. In O. Neuman, F. Sanders (Eds.), Aufmerksamkeit[Attention] (pp. 479–558). Göttingen: Hogrefe First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yantis, S. , Schwarzbach, J. , Serences, J.T. , Carlson, R.L. , Steinmetz, M.A. , Pekar, J.J. (2002). Transient neural activity in human parietal cortex during spatial attention shifts Nature Neuroscience, 5, 995–1002. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yantis, S. , Serences, J.T. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 187–193. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar