Skip to main content
Article

Affective Context of Sadness and Physiological Response Patterns

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000031

This study was conducted to test the hypothesis that two films that induce a sad feeling would elicit different physiological responses depending on an additional context of the film contents related to either avoidance (disgust) or attachment (tenderness). Reactivity was evaluated for facial behavior, heart rate, pulse transit time, skin conductance, and subjective experience. Participants reported feeling less happy and showed increased facial activity related to the sad content of both films. The sad film related to avoidance induced an increase in skin conductance level and response rate. In contrast, the sad film related to attachment induced a decrease in amplitude of skin conductance responses and heart rate. The study showed that while the common sad content of both films disturbed mood or provoked negative feelings, additional affective contexts induced either a decrease or an increase in physiological arousal.

References

  • Baumgartner, T. , Esslen, M. , Jäncke, L. (2006). From emotion perception to emotion experience: emotions evoked by pictures and classical music. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 34–43. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Boucsein, W. (1992). Electrodermal activity. New York, NY: Plenum Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M. , Codispoti, M. , Cuthbert, B. N. , Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buitelaar, J. K. (1995). Attachment and social withdrawal in autism: Hypotheses and findings. Behaviour, 132, 319–350. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davydov, D. M. , Shapiro, D. , Goldstein, I. B. (2010). Relation of resting baroreflex activity to 24-hour blood pressure and mood in healthy subjects. Journal of Psychophysiology, 24, 149–160. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Davydov, D. M. , Shapiro, D. , Cook, I. A. , Goldstein, I. B. (2007). Baroreflex mechanisms in major depression. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 31, 164–177. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davydov, D. M. , Shapiro, D. , Goldstein, I. B. , Chicz-DeMet, A. (2005). Moods in everyday situations: Effects of menstrual cycle, work, and stress hormones. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 58, 343–349. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davydov, D. M. , Shapiro, D. , Goldstein, I. B. , Chicz-DeMet, A. (2007). Moods in everyday situations: Effects of combination of different arousal-related factors. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 62, 321–329. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Jong, P. J. , Peters, M. , Vanderhallen, I. (2002). Disgust and disgust sensitivity in spider phobia: Facial EMG in response to spider and oral disgust imagery. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16, 477–493. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. (1989). The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions of emotion. In H. Wagner, A. Manstead (Eds.), Handbook of social psychophysiology (pp. 143–164). London, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. (2003). Darwin, deception, and facial expression. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1000, 205–221. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Felblinger, J. , Boesch, C. (1997). Amplitude demodulation of the electrocardiogram signal (ECG) for respiration monitoring and compensation during MR examinations. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 38, 129–136. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fredrickson, B. L. , Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 12, 191–220. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fridlund, A. J. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, B. H. , Allen, M. T. , Christie, I. C. , Santucci, A. K. (2002). Quantification issues in time- and frequency-domain measures of heart rate variability. IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine, 21, 35–40. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gold, P. W. , Chrousos, G. P. (2002). Organization of the stress system and its dysregulation in melancholic and atypical depression: high vs low CRH/NE states. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 254–275. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gračanin, A. , Kardum, I. , Hudek-Knežević, J. (2007). Relations between dispositional expressivity and physiological changes during acute positive and negative affect. Psychological Topics, 16, 311–328. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Koelsch, S. , Remppis, A. , Sammler, D. , Jentschke, S. , Mietchen, D. , Fritz, T. , Bonnemeier, H. , Siebel, W. A. (2007). A cardiac signature of emotionality. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 3328–3338. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kreibig, S. D. , Wilhelm, F. H. , Roth, W. T. , Gross, J. J. (2007). Cardiovascular, electrodermal, and respiratory response patterns to fear- and sadness-inducing films. Psychophysiology, 44, 787–806. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J. , Bradley, M. M. , Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. Gainesville, FL: NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lovell, D. (Producer) (1979). F. Zeffirelli (Director) (1979) The Champ [Film]. Culver City, CA: MGM/UA. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Luminet, O. , Bouts, P. , Delie, F. , Manstead, A. S. R. , Rimé, B. (2000). Social sharing of emotion following exposure to a negatively valenced situation. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 661–688. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mauss, I. B. , Levenson, R. W. , McCarter, L. , Wilhelm, F. H. , Gross, J. J. (2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion, 5, 175–190. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Medford, N. , Phillips, M. L. , Brierley, B. , Brammer, M. , Bullmore, E. T. , David, A. S. (2005). Emotional memory: Separating content and context. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 138, 247–258. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ortony, A. , Turner, T. J. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions?. Psychological Review, 97, 315–331. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ottaviani, C. , Shapiro, D. , Davydov, D. M. , Goldstein, I. B. (2008). Autonomic stress response modes and ambulatory heart rate level and variability. Journal of Psychophysiology, 22, 28–40. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Pollak, M. H. , Obrist, P. A. (1983). Aortic-radial pulse transit time and ECG Q-wave to radial pulse wave interval as indices of beat-by-beat blood pressure change. Psychophysiology, 20, 21–28. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rimé, B. , Finkenauer, C. , Luminet, O. , Zech, E. , Philippot, P. (1998). Social sharing of emotion: New evidence and new questions. In W. Stroebe, M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology, (Vol. 9, pp. 145–189). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rimé, B. , Philippot, P. , Boca, S. , Mesquita, B. (1992). Long-lasting cognitive and social consequences of emotion: Social sharing and rumination. In W. Stroebe, M. Hewstone (Eds.), European Review of Social Psychology, (Vol. 3, pp. 225–258). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roberts, N. A. , Levenson, R. W. , Gross, J. J. (2008). Cardiovascular costs of emotion suppression cross ethnic lines. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70, 82–87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rohrmann, S. , Hopp, H. (2008). Cardiovascular indicators of disgust. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68, 201–208. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rozin, P. (1999). Preadaptation and the puzzles and properties of pleasure. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well being: The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 109–133). New York, NY: Russell Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schaefer, A. , Collette, F. , Philippot, P. , Vanderlinden, M. , Laureys, S. , Delfiore, G. , …, Salmon, E. (2003). Neural correlates of “hot” and “cold” emotional processing: A multilevel approach to the functional anatomy of emotions. Neuroimage, 18, 938–949. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schaefer, A. , Nils, F. , Sanchez, X. , Philippot, P. (2008). Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: A new tool for emotion researchers. Technical report. University of Louvain, 10 Place du Cardinal Mercier, 1348-Louvain-La-Neuve, Belgium. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Scherer, K. R. , Schorr, A. , Johnstone, T. (2001). Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shapiro, D. , Jamner, L. D. , Goldstein, I. B. , Delfino, R. J. (2001). Striking a chord: Moods, blood pressure, and heart rate in everyday life. Psychophysiology, 38, 197–204. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stark, R. , Stone, A. , White, V. (Producers), & Ross, H. (Director) (1989). Steel Magnolias [Film]. Culver City, CA:TriStar Pictures. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stemmler, G. , Aue, T. , Wacker, J. (2007). Anger and fear: Separable effects of emotion and motivational direction on somatovisceral responses. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66, 141–153. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stemmler, G. , Heldmann, M. , Pauls, C. A. , Scherer, T. (2001). Constraints for emotion specificity in fear and anger: The context counts. Psychophysiology, 38, 275–291. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Uryvaev, Iu. V. , Davydov, D. M. , Gavrilenko, A. Ia. (1988). An improved method of speech audiometry. Vestnik Otorinolaringologii [Bulletin of Otolaryngology], 3, 34–39. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Uryvaev, Iu. V. , Davydov, D. M. , Gavrilenko, A. Ia. (1991). Study of food dominants by means of a verbal test. Fiziologiia Cheloveka [Human Physiology], 17, 67–72. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Van der Does, W. (2002). Different types of experimentally induced sad mood?. Behavior Therapy, 33, 551–561. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Gucht, D. , Vansteenwegen, D. , Beckers, T. , Hermans, D. , Baeyens, F. , Van den Bergh, O. (2008). Repeated cue exposure effects on subjective and physiological indices of chocolate craving. Appetite, 50, 19–24. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Volokhov, R. N. , Demaree, H. A. (2010). Spontaneous emotion regulation to positive and negative stimuli. Brain and Cognition, 73, 1–6. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wallbott, H. G. , Scherer, K. R. (1986). How universal and specific is emotional experience? Evidence from 27 countries on five continents. Social Science Information, 25, 763–795. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D. , Clark, L. A. , Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zech, E. , Rimé, B. (2005). Is talking about an emotional experience helpful? Effects on emotional recovery and perceived benefits. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 12, 270–287. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar