Skip to main content
Article

How Fast is Fear?

Automatic and Controlled Processing in Conditioned Fear

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000063

The minimum latency of potentiated startle after delay and trace fear conditioning was investigated. Delay conditioning is hypothesized to be mediated by automatic processes, whereas trace conditioning is hypothesized to involve controlled cognitive processes. In a group receiving delay conditioning, a tone conditioned stimulus (CS) signaled an electric shock unconditioned stimulus (US) presented 1,000 ms after CS onset. In a group receiving trace conditioning, a 200 ms tone CS was followed by an 800 ms gap prior to US presentation. Two control groups received unpaired CS/US presentations. It was hypothesized that fear-potentiated startle should be observed at shorter time intervals after CS onset in the group receiving delay conditioning compared to the group receiving trace conditioning. The results showed increased startle at 100 and 150 ms after CS onset in the group receiving delay conditioning compared to the unpaired group. In the group receiving trace conditioning, increased startle was observed at 1,500 ms after CS onset compared to the unpaired group. This supports the idea that conditioned fear after delay conditioning may be due to automatic processes, whereas trace conditioning is dependent on controlled processes.

References

  • Åsli, O. , Flaten, M. A. (2008). Conditioned facilitation of the unconditioned reflex after classical eyeblink conditioning. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 67, 17–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Åsli, O. , Kulvedrøsten, S. , Solbakken, L. E. , Flaten, M. A. (2009). Fear potentiated startle at short intervals following conditioned stimulus onset during delay but not trace conditioning. Psychophysiology, 46, 880–888. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blumenthal, T. D. , Elden, Å. , Flaten, M. A. (2004). A comparison of several methods used to quantify prepulse inhibition of eyeblink responding. Psychophysiology, 41, 326–332. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burman, M. A. , Gewirtz, J. C. (2004). Timing of fear expression in trace and delay conditioning measured by fear-potentiated startle in rats. Learning & Memory, 11, 205–212. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burman, M. A. , Gewirtz, J. C. (2007). Hippocampal activity, but not plasticity, is required for early consolidation of fear conditioning with a short trace interval. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2483–2490. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, C. K. Y. , Lovibond, P. F. (1996). Expectancy bias in trait anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology November, 105, 637–647. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, R. E. , Squire, L. R. (1998). Classical conditioning and brain systems: The role of awareness. Science, 280, 77–81. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, R. E. , Squire, L. R. (1999). Human eyeblink classical conditioning: Effects of manipulating awareness of the stimulus contingencies. Psychological Science, 10, 14–18. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davis, M. (2000). The role of the amygdala in conditioned and unconditioned fear and anxiety. In J. P. Aggelton (Ed.), The amygdala (2nd ed., pp. 213–287). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Davis, M. , Schlesinger, L. S. , Sorenson, C. A. (1989). Temporal specificity of fear conditioning: Effects of different conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus intervals on the fear-potentiated startle effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 15, 295–310. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elden, Å. , Flaten, M. A. (2002). The relationship of automatic and controlled processing to prepulse inhibition. Journal of Psychophysiology, 16, 46–55. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Elden, Å. , Flaten, M. A. (2003). Similar effects of attention directed to acoustic and tactile stimuli on prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44, 361–370. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fanselow, M. S. , Kim, J. J. (1994). Acquisition of contextual Pavlovian fear conditioning is blocked by application of an NMDA receptor antagonist D, L-2-Amino-5-Phosphonovaleric acid to the basolateral amygdala. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 210–212. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Filion, D. L. , Dawson, M. E. , Schell, A. M. (1993). Modification of the acoustic startle-reflex eyeblink: A tool for investigating early and late attentional processes. Biological Psychology, 35, 185–200. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Filion, D. L. , Poje, A. B. (2003). Selective and nonselective attention effects on prepulse inhibition of startle: A comparison of task and no-task protocols. Biological Psychology, 64, 283–296. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flaten, M. A. (1993). Startle reflex facilitation as a function of classical eyeblink conditioning in humans. Psychophysiology, 30, 581–588. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flaten, M. A. , Elden, Å. (1999). Caffeine and prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Psychopharmacology, 147, 322–330. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gabrieli, J. D. E. , Carrillo, M. C. , Cermak, L. S. , McGlinchey-Berroth, R. , Gluck, M. A. , Disterhoft, J. F. (1995). Intact delay-eyeblink classical conditioning in amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 819–827. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graham, F. K. (1975). More or less startling effects of weak pre-stimulation. Psychophysiology, 12, 238–248. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hamm, A. O. , Vaitl, D. (1996). Affective learning: Awareness and aversion. Psychophysiology, 33, 698–710. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hardwick, S. A. , Lipp, O. V. (2000). Modulation of affective learning: An occasion for evaluative conditioning? Learning and Motivation, 31, 251–271. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Helmstetter, F. J. , Bellgowan, P. S. (1994). Hypoalgesia in response to sensitization during acute noise stress. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 177–185. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jovanovic, T. E. , Keyes, M. , Fiallos, A. , Myers, K. M. , Davis, M. , Duncan, E. J. (2005). Fear potentiation and fear inhibition in a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1559–1564. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jovanovic, T. E. , Norrholm, S. D. , Keyes, M. , Fiallos, A. , Jovanovic, S. , Myers, K. M. , … Duncan, E. J. (2006). Contingency awareness and fear inhibition in a human fear-potentiated startle paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 995–1004. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kehoe, E. J. , Napier, R. M. (1991). In the blink of an eye – real-time stimulus factors in delay and trace conditioning of the rabbits nictitating-membrane response. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B – Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 257–277. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Koch, M. (1999). The neurobiology of startle. Progress in Neurobiology, 59, 107–128. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LaBar, K. S. , LeDoux, J. E. (1996). Partial disruption of fear conditioning in rats with unilateral amygdala damage: Correspondence with unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 110, 991–997. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J. , Bradley, M. , Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97, 377–395. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Emotional memory systems in the brain. Behavioural Brain Research, 58, 69–79. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • LeDoux, J. E. , Farb, C. R. , Romanski, L. M. (1991). Overlapping projections to the amygdala and striatum from auditory processing areas of the thalamus and cortex. Neuroscience Letters, 134, 139–144. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lovibond, P. F. , Shanks, D. R. (2002). The role of awareness in Pavlovian conditioning: Empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 3–26. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Manns, J. R. , Clark, R. E. , Squire, L. R. (2000). Awareness predicts the magnitude of single-cue trace eyeblink conditioning. Hippocampus, 10, 181–186. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Manns, J. R. , Clark, R. E. , Squire, L. R. (2001). Single-cue delay eyeblink conditioning is unrelated to awareness. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 1, 192–198. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mascagni, F. , McDonald, A. J. , Coleman, J. R. (1993). Corticoamygdaloid and corticocortical projections of the rat temporal cortex – a phaseolus-vulgaris-leukoagglutinin study. Neuroscience, 57, 697–715. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McEchron, M. D. , Bouwmeester, H. , Tseng, W. , Weiss, C. , Disterhoft, J. F. (1998). Hippocampectomy disrupts auditory trace fear conditioning and contextual fear conditioning in the rat. Hippocampus, 8, 638–646. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McGlinchey-Berroth, R. , Carrillo, M. C. , Gabrieli, J. D. E. , Brawn, C. M. , Disterhoft, J. F. (1997). Impaired trace eyeblink conditioning in bilateral, medial-temporal lobe amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 873–882. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miserendino, M. J. D. , Sananes, C. B. , Melia, K. R. , Davis, M. (1990). Blocking of acquisition but not expression of conditioned fear-potentiated startle by NMDA antagonists in the amygdala. Nature, 345, 716–718. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Öhman, A. , Soares, J. J. F. (1998). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli: Expectancies for aversive outcomes following nonrecognized fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 69–82. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quinn, J. J. , Oommen, S. S. , Morrison, G. E. , Fanselow, M. S. (2002). Post-training excitotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus attenuate forward trace, backward trace, and delay fear conditioning in a temporally specific manner. Hippocampus, 12, 495–504. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schell, A. M. , Wynn, J. K. , Dawson, M. E. , Sinaii, N. (2000). Automatic and controlled attentional processes in startle eyeblink modification: Effects of habituation of the prepulse. Psychophysiology, 37, 409–417. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silverstein, L. D. , Graham, F. K. , Calloway, J. M. (1980). Preconditioning and excitability of the human orbicularis oculi reflex as a function of state. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 48, 406–417. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, C. N. , Clark, R. E. , Manns, J. R. , Squire, L. R. (2005). Acquisition of differential delay eyeblink classical conditioning is independent of awareness. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 78–86. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Solomon, P. R. , Vander Schaaf, E. R. , Thompson, R. F. , Weisz, D. J. (1986). Hippocampus and trace conditioning of the rabbit’s classically conditioned nictitating membrane response. Behavioral Neuroscience, 100, 729–744. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weike, A. I. , Schupp, H. T. , Hamm, A. O. (2008). In dubio pro defensio: Initial activation of conditioned fear is not cue specific. Behavioral Neuroscience, 122, 685–696. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Woodruff-Pak, D. S. (1993). Eyeblink classical conditioning in H M: Delay and trace paradigms. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 911–925. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar