Skip to main content
Article

N400 Peculiarities During Intentional Inferences Production in Subjects With Schizotypal Traits

Preserved Predictions of Intentions but Altered Context Integration

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000292

Abstract. A deficit in context processing may underlie theory of mind (ToM) difficulties in schizophrenia, although few studies to date have explored the impact of contextual processing on ToM performances within the same task. We used electroencephalography to investigate the production of intentional inferences from highly versus weakly structured sentences in healthy participants with schizotypal traits. Thirty-four participants were divided into two groups according to their scores on the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ). The participants listened to stories that required a degree of understanding in order to produce an inference about the main character’s intention. Each story was followed by a target word that could either be highly predictable (HP), weakly predictable (WP), or incompatible with the context. The results indicate that the N400 component for WP targets was stronger in the high-SPQ group. This increase correlated with the negative dimension of schizotypy. This may reflect difficulties with generating intentional inferences when the context is insufficiently structured for high schizotypy participants.

References

  • Abu-Akel, A., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2013). Characteristics of theory of mind Impairments in schizophrenia. In D. L. RobertsD. L. PennEds., Social cognition in schizophrenia (pp. 196–214). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/med:psych/9780199777587.003.0008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aldebot Sacks, S., Weisman de Mamani, A. G., & Garcia, C. P. (2012). Associations between cognitive biases and domains of schizotypy in a non-clinical sample. Psychiatry Research, 196(1), 115–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.09.019 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baez, S., Herrera, E., Villarin, L., Theil, D., Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Gomez, P., Mosquera, M., Huepe, D., Strejilevich, S., Vigliecca, N. S., Matthäus, F., Decety, J., Manes, F., & Ibañez, A. M. (2013). Contextual social cognition impairments in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. PLoS One, 8(3), Article e57664. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0057664 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Binois, R., & Pichot, P. (1956). Test de vocabulaire: manuel d’application [Vocabulary test: Application manual]. Editions du Centre de psychologie appliquée. https://books.google.fr/books?id=1H3rXwAACAAJ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bora, E. (2020). Theory of mind and schizotypy: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 222, 97–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2020.04.024 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brunet-Gouet, E., Achim, A. M., Vistoli, D., Passerieux, C., Hardy-Baylé, M.-C., & Jackson, P. L. (2011). The study of social cognition with neuroimaging methods as a means to explore future directions of deficit evaluation in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Research, 190(1), 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.11.029 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Champagne-Lavau, M., & Charest, A. (2015). Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of social knowledge. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, Article 98. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00098 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Champagne-Lavau, M., Charest, A., Anselmo, K., Rodriguez, J.-P., & Blouin, G. (2012). Theory of mind and context processing in schizophrenia: The role of cognitive flexibility. Psychiatry Research, 200(2–3), 184–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.011 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chung, Y. S., Kang, D.-H., Shin, N. Y., Yoo, S. Y., & Kwon, J. S. (2008). Deficit of theory of mind in individuals at ultra-high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 99(1–3), 111–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, A. S., Mohr, C., Ettinger, U., Chan, R. C. K., & Park, S. (2015). Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(suppl 2), S427–S435. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu195 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., & Roberts, D. L. (2006). The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(Supplement 1), S44–S63. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl029 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Del Goleto, S., Kostova, M., & Blanchet, A. (2016). Impaired context processing during irony comprehension in schizotypy: An ERPs study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 105, 17–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.04.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Loye, C., Beaucousin, V., Bohec, A.-L., Blanchet, A., & Kostova, M. (2013). An event-related potential study of predictive and integrative semantic context processing in subjects with schizotypal traits. Psychophysiology, 50(11), 1109–1119. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12124 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dumas, P., Bouafia, S., Gutknecht, C., Saoud, M., Dalery, J., & d’Amato, T. (2000). Validation of the French version of the Raine Schizotypal Personality Disorder Questionnaire – categorial and dimensional approach to schizotypal personality traits in a normal student population. L’Encéphale, 26(5), 23–29. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ettinger, U., Meyhöfer, I., Steffens, M., Wagner, M., & Koutsouleris, N. (2014). Genetics, cognition, and neurobiology of schizotypal personality: A review of the overlap with schizophrenia. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, Article 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). Right words and left words: Electrophysiological evidence for hemispheric differences in meaning processing. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 8(3), 373–392. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0926-6410(99)00036-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fett, A.-K. J., Viechtbauer, W., Dominguez, M.-G., Penn, D. L., van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2011). The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 573–588. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gooding, D. C., & Pflum, M. J. (2011). Theory of mind and psychometric schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 188(2), 217–223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.029 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. A. (1983). New method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55(4), 468–484. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(83)90135-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, M. F., Penn, D. L., Bentall, R., Carpenter, W. T., Gaebel, W., Gur, R. C., Kring, A. M., Park, S., Silverstein, S. M., & Heinssen, R. (2008). Social cognition in schizophrenia: An NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(6), 1211–1220. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm145 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, M. J., Uhlhaas, P., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Context processing and social cognition in schizophrenia. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 1(1), 11–22. https://doi.org/10.2174/1573400052953529 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hardy-Baylé, M.-C., Sarfati, Y., & Passerieux, C. (2003). The cognitive basis of disorganization symptomatology in schizophrenia and its clinical correlates: Toward a pathogenetic approach to disorganization. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(3), 459–471. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Henry, J. D., Bailey, P. E., & Rendell, P. G. (2008). Empathy, social functioning and schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 160(1), 15–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.04.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kiang, M. (2010). Schizotypy and language: A review. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23(3), 193–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.03.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kiang, M., & Kutas, M. (2005). Association of schizotypy with semantic processing differences: An event-related brain potential study. Schizophrenia Research, 77(2–3), 329–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.03.021 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kostova, M., Bohec, A.-L., & Blanchet, A. (2014). Event-related brain potential study of expectancy and semantic matching in schizotypy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 92(2), 67–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.02.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kostova, M., Passerieux, C., Laurent, J. P., Saint-Georges, C., & Hardy-Baylé, M. C. (2003). Functional analysis of the deficit in semantic context processes in schizophrenic patients: An event-related potentials study. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology, 33(1), 11–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kuperberg, G. R., Kreher, D. A., & Ditman, T. (2010). What can event-related potentials tell us about language, and perhaps even thought, in schizophrenia? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 75(2), 66–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.09.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding Meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 621–647. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (1999). Mentalising, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. Cognition, 71(1), 43–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(99)00018-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lavoie, M.-A., Plana, I., Bédard Lacroix, J., Godmaire-Duhaime, F., Jackson, P. L., & Achim, A. M. (2013). Social cognition in first-degree relatives of people with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 209(2), 129–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.11.037 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mohammad, O. M., & DeLisi, L. E. (2013). N400 in schizophrenia patients. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 26(2), 196–207. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e32835d9e56 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morrison, S. C., Brown, L. A., & Cohen, A. S. (2013). A multidimensional assessment of social cognition in psychometrically defined schizotypy. Psychiatry Research, 210(3), 1014–1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prévost, M., Rodier, M., Lionnet, C., Brodeur, M., King, S., & Debruille, J. B. (2011). Paranoid induction reduces N400s of healthy subjects with delusional-like ideation: Paranoid induction reduces N400s. Psychophysiology, 48(7), 937–949. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01160.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prévost, M., Rodier, M., Renoult, L., Kwann, Y., Dionne-Dostie, E., Chapleau, I., Brodeur, M., Lionnet, C., & Debruille, J. B. (2010). Schizotypal traits and N400 in healthy subjects. Psychophysiology, 47(6), 1047–1056. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01016.x First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Raine, A. (1991). The SPQ: A scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17(4), 555–564. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/17.4.555 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rapp, A. M., Mutschler, D. E., Wild, B., Erb, M., Lengsfeld, I., Saur, R., & Grodd, W. (2010). Neural correlates of irony comprehension: The role of schizotypal personality traits. Brain and Language, 113(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.11.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rinaldi, R., Lefebvre, L., Blekic, W., Laroi, F., & Laloyaux, J. (2018). Attribution of intentions and context processing in psychometric schizotypy. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 23(6), 364–376. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2018.1528972 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roux, P., Brunet-Gouet, E., Passerieux, C., & Ramus, F. (2016). Eye-tracking reveals a slowdown of social context processing during intention attribution in patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 41(2), E13–E21. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.150045 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Savla, G. N., Vella, L., Armstrong, C. C., Penn, D. L., & Twamley, E. W. (2013). Deficits in Domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 39(5), 979–992. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs080 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schenkel, L., Spaulding, W., & Silverstein, S. (2005). Poor premorbid social functioning and theory of mind deficit in schizophrenia: Evidence of reduced context processing? Journal of Psychiatric Research, 39(5), 499–508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.01.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Harnett, K., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., Hergueta, T., Baker, R., & Dunbar, G. C. (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 22–33. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Uhlhaas, P. J., Phillips, W. A., Schenkel, L. S., & Silverstein, S. M. (2006). Theory of mind and perceptual context‐processing in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 11(4), 416–436. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800444000272 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Varga, E., Simon, M., Tényi, T., Schnell, Z., Hajnal, A., Orsi, G., Dóczi, T., Komoly, S., Janszky, J., Füredi, R., Hamvas, E., Fekete, S., & Herold, R. (2013). Irony comprehension and context processing in schizophrenia during remission – A functional MRI study. Brain and Language, 126(3), 231–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, K., Cheung, E. F. C., Gong, Q., & Chan, R. C. K. (2011). Semantic processing disturbance in patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of the N400 component. PLoS One, 6(10), Article e25435. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025435 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, Y., Liu, W., Li, Z., Wei, X., Jiang, X., Neumann, D. L., Shum, D. H. K., Cheung, E. F. C., & Chan, R. C. K. (2015). Dimensional schizotypy and social cognition: An fMRI imaging study. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, Article 133. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00133 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar