Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.30.1.28

Several lines of evidence attest robust relationships between personality dimensions and emotions, including cognitive aspect of emotion. More particularly, many studies reported strong relationships between extraversion, the behavioral activation system (BAS), and the cognitive processing of positive information, on the one hand, and between neuroticism, the behavioral inhibition system (BIS), and the processing of negative information, on the other hand. Recently, DePascalis, Awari, Matteucci, and Mazzocco (2005) reported that personality traits modulated the effect of the emotional visual stimuli on the mismatch negativity (MMN). The aim of the present study was to replicate these data and extend them to other personality dimensions. Auditory MMN was recorded in normal subjects simultaneously to the presentation of emotional pictures selected as neutral, positive, or negative from the International Affective Picture System, and presented in randomized order. The results support the recent finding that personality (namely, BIS and harm avoidance) modulates the influence of emotional (negative) context on auditory information processing. The present findings suggest that the modulation by personality of change detection in the unattended environment as a function of context valence is limited to unpleasant context.

References

  • Amrhein, C. , Mühlberger, A. , Pauli, P. , Wiedemann, G. (2004). Modulation of event-related brain potentials during affective picture processing: A complement to startle reflex and skin conductance response? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 54, 231–240. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, B.P. , Mogg, K. (1994). Mood and personality in recall of positive and negative information. Behavior Research and Therapy, 32, 137–141. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Canli, T. , Sivers, H. , Withfield, S.L. , Gotlib, I.H. , Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2002). Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science, 296, 2191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C.S. , White, T.L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impeding reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention [CSEA-NIMH] . (1999). International affective picture system: Digitalized photographs. Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cloninger, C.R. (1987). A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cloninger, C.R. (1999). The temperament and character inventory-revised. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cloninger, C.R. , Svrakic, D.M. , Przybeck, T.R. (1993). A psychobiological model of temperaments and character. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Corr, P.J. (2002). J.A. Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory: Tests of the joint subsystem hypothesis of anxiety and impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 511–532. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Costa, P.T. , McCrae, R.R. (1990). The NEO Personality Inventory manual revised. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • DePascalis, V. , Awari, B. , Matteucci, A. , Mazzocco, A. (2005). Effect of emotional visual stimuli on auditory information processing: A test of J.A. Gray’s reinforcement sensitivity theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 163–178. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DePascalis, V. , Speranza, O. (2000). Personality effects on attentional shift to emotional charged cues: ERP, behavioral and HR data. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 217–238. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DePascalis, V. , Strippoli, E. , Riccardi, P. , Vergari, F. (2004). Personality, event-related potential (ERP), and hear rate (HR) in emotional word processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 873–891. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Delplanque, S. , Silvert, L. , Hot, P. , Sequeira, H. (2006). Event-related P3a and P3b in response to unpredictable emotional stimuli. Biological Psychology, 68, 107–20. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fabiani, M. , Gratton, G. , Coles, M.G.H. (2000). Event-related brain potentials: Methods, theory, and applications. In J.T. Cacioppo L.G. Tassinary G.G. Bernston, Eds., Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 53–84). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Franken, I.H. , Nijs, I. , VanStrien, J.W. (2005). Impulsivity affects mismatch negativity (MMN) measures of preattentive auditory processing. Biological Psychology, 70, 161–167. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gable, S.L. , Reis, H.T. , Elliot, A.J. (2000). Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1135–1149. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gomez, A. , Gomez, R. (2002). Personality traits of the behavioral approach and inhibition systems: Associations with processing of emotional stimuli. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1299–1316. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gross, J.J. , Sutton, S.K. , Ketelaar, T. (1998). Relations between affect and personality: Support for the affect-level and affective-reactivity views. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 279–288. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hansenne, M. , Pinto, E. , Scantamburlo, G. , Renard, B. , Reggers, J. , Fuchs, S. et al. (2003). Harm avoidance is related to mismatch negativity (MMN) amplitude in healthy subjects. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1039–1048. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heponiemi, T. , Keltikangas-Järvinen, L., Puttonen, S., Ravaja, N. (2003). BIS/BAS sensitivity and self-rated affects during experimentally induced stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 943–957. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P.J. , Greenwald, M.K. , Bradley, M.M. , Hamm, A.O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology, 30, 261–273. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. (2000). Mismatch negativity (MMN): Perspectives for application. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 37, 3–10. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R. (2001). The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent (MMNm). Psychophysiology, 38, 1–21. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A.W., Mantysalo, S. (1978). Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica, 42, 313–329. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Palomba, D. , Angrilli, A. , Mini, A. (1997). Visual evoked potentials, heart rate responses, and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, 55–67. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Picton, T.W. , Alain, C. , Otten, L. , Ritter, W. , Achim, A. (2000). Mismatch negativity: Different water in the same river. Audiology and Neurootology, 5, 111–139. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rusting, C.L. (1999). Interactive effects of personality and mood on emotion-congruent memory and judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1073–1086. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sasaki, T. , Campbell, K.B. , Gordon Bazana, P. , Stelmack, R.M. (2000). Individual differences in mismatch negativity measures of involuntary attention shift. Clinical Neurophysiology, 111, 1553–1560. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Surakka, V. , Tenhunen-Eskelinen, M. , Hietanen, J.K. , Sams, M. (1998). Modulation of human auditory information processing by emotional visual stimuli. Cognitive and Brain Research, 7, 159–163. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Torrubia, R. , Avila, C. , Moltó, J.M. , Caseras, X. (2001). The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 837–862. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van de Velde, M. , Ghosh, I.R. , Cluitmans, P.J. (1999). Context-related artefact detection in prolonged EEG recordings. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 60, 183–196. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, W. , Zhu, S.Z. , Pan, L.C. , Hu, A.H. , Wang, Y.H. (2001). Mismatch negativity and personality traits in chronic primary insomniacs. Functional Neurology, 16, 3–10. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Winkler, I. , Cowan, N. (2005). From sensory to long-term memory: Evidence from auditory memory reactivation studies. Experimental Psychology, 52, 3–20. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Winkler, I. , Cowan, N. , Csépe, V. , Czigler, I. , Näätänen, R. (1996). Interactions between transient and long-term auditory memory as reflected by the mismatch negativity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 403–415. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar