Skip to main content
Original Article

Spatial Tests, Familiarity with the Surroundings, and Spatial Activity Experience

How Do They Contribute to Children’s Spatial Orientation in Macro Environments?

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000010

Spatial orientation as the ability to know the bearing to the origin of a walked path was investigated in two studies with ca. 140 preschool and primary school children who walked paths of about 1 km beginning at the familiar kindergarten or in a completely unknown territory. Path difficulty and familiarity with the surroundings influenced correctness of pointing. Spatial ability measured by test performance and spatial activity experience, i.e., children’s reports about unsupervised walks, effected pointing accuracy as well. The data emphasize that spatial activity experience may be an important factor for spatial orientation beyond kindergarten age.

References

  • Anooshian, L.J. , Nelson, S.K. (1987). Children’s knowledge of directional relationships within their neighborhood. Cognitive Development, 2, 113–126. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carroll, J.B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coluccia, E. , Louse, G. (2004). Gender differences in spatial orientation: A review. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 24, 329–340. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Davies, C. , & Uttal, D.H. (2007). Map use and the development of spatial cognition. In J.M. Plumert J.P. Spencer, Eds., The emerging spatial mind (pp. 219–247). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Etienne, A.S. (1992). Navigation of a small mammal by dead reckoning and local cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1, 48–52. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fenner, J. , Heathcote, D. , Jerrams-Smith, J. (2000). The development of wayfinding competency: Asymmetrical effects of visuo-spatial and verbal ability. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 20, 165–175. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gärling, T., Selart, M. , Böök, A. (1997). Investigating spatial choice and navigation in large-scale environments. In N. Foreman R. Gillett, Eds., A handbook of spatial research paradigms and methodologies (pp. 153–180). Hove: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Huttenlocher, J., Newcombe, N. , Vasilyeva, M. (1999). Spatial scaling in young children. Psychological Science, 10, 393–398. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kearns, M.J., Warren, W.H. , Duchon, A.P. , Tarr, M. (2002). Path integration from optic flow and body senses in a homing task. Perception, 31, 349–374. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kirasic, K.C. (2000). Age differences in adults’ spatial abilities, learning environmental layout, and way-finding behavior. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 2, 117–134. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lehnung, M., Haaland, V.O., Pohl, J. , Leplow, B. (2001). Compass- versus finger-pointing tasks: The influence of different methods of assessment on age-related orientation performance in children. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 21, 283–289. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lehnung, M., Leplow, B. , Haaland, V.O. , Mehdorn, M. , Ferstl, R. (2003). Pointing accuracy in children is dependent on age, sex and experience. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 23, 419–425. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leplow, B. , Lehnung, M., Pohl, J., Herzog, A., Ferstl, R. , Mehdorn, M. (2003). Navigational place learning in children and young adults as assessed with a standardized locomotor search task. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 299–317. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neidhardt, E. (2002). Orientierung bei Vorschulkindern: Zwei Feldexperimente zur Pfadintegration [Orientation in preschool children: Two field experiments on path integration]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 34, 185–193. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Neidhardt, E. (2004). Die ontogenetische Entwicklung von Raumkognition in Makroräumen – Pfadintegration bei Vorschul- und Grundschulkindern [Ontogenetic development of spatial cognition in macro spatial environments – Path integration in preschool and primary school children]. Habilitationsschrift, Universität Marburg. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Neidhardt, E. , Schmitz, S. (2001). Entwicklung von Strategien und Kompetenzen in der räumlichen Orientierung und in der Raumkognition: Einflüsse von Geschlecht, Alter, Erfahrung und Motivation [Development of strategies and competences in spatial orientation and in spatial cognition: Influence of sex, age, experience, and motivation]. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht, 48, 262–279. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Newcombe, N.S. , Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Plester, B., Richards, J., Blades, M. , Spencer, C. (2002). Young children’s ability to use aerial photographs as maps. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22, 29–47. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Popp, M.M. , Platzer, E., Eichner, M. , Schade, M. (2004). Walking with and without walking: Perception of distance in large-scale urban areas in reality and in virtual reality. Presence, 13, 61–76. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rider, E.A. , Rieser, J.J. (1988). Pointing at objects in other rooms: Young children’s sensitivity to perspective after walking with and without vision. Child Development, 59, 480–494. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rieser, J.J. , Pick, H.L., Jr. (2007). Using locomotion to update spatial orientation: What changes with learning and development? In J.M. Plumert J.P. Spencer, Eds., The emerging spatial mind (pp. 77–103). New York: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spencer, C. , Darvizeh, Z. (1983). Young children’s place-descriptions, maps and route-finding: A comparison of nursery school children in Iran and Britain. International Journal of Early Childhood, 15, 26–31. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thurstone, L.L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Psychometric monographs (Vol. 1). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weatherford, D.L. (1985). Representing and manipulating spatial information from different environments: Models to neighborhoods. In R. Cohen, (Ed.), The development of spatial cognition (pp. 41–70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar