Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000228

We examined whether the generalization of recently acquired likes and dislikes depends on feature-specific attention allocation. Likes and dislikes were established by means of an evaluative-conditioning procedure in which participants were presented with several exemplars of two subordinate categories (e.g., young men vs. old women). Whereas exemplars of one category were consistently paired with negative stimuli, exemplars of the second category were consistently paired with positive stimuli. In addition, we manipulated feature-specific attention allocation for specific stimulus dimensions (e.g., gender vs. age), either during (Experiments 1 and 2) or before the acquisition phase of the experiment (Experiment 3). Both direct and indirect attitude measures revealed a clear impact of this manipulation on attitude generalization. More specifically, only generalization stimuli that were similar to the CSs in terms of the stimulus dimension that was selectively attended to were evaluated in a manner that was congruent with the acquired liking of those CSs.

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1935). Attitudes. In C. Murchison (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 798–844). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baeyens, F. , Eelen, P. , Crombez, G. , Van den Bergh, O. (1992). Human evaluative conditioning: Acquisition trials, presentation schedule, evaluative style and contingency awareness. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 133–142. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(92)90136-5 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Baeyens, F. , Kaes, B. , Eelen, P. , Silverans, P. (1996). Observational evaluative conditioning of an embedded stimulus element. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 15–28. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199601)26:1<15::AID-EJSP729>3.0.CO;2-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bar-Anan, Y. , De Houwer, J. , Nosek, B. A. (2010). Evaluative conditioning and conscious knowledge of contingencies: A correlational investigation with large samples. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2313–2335. doi: 10.1080/17470211003802442 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bar-Anan, Y. , Nosek, B. A. (2012). Reporting intentional rating of the primes predicts priming effects in the affective misattribution procedure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 1194–1208. doi: 10.1177/0146167212446835 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bierley, C. , McSweeney, F. K. , Vannieuwkerk, R. (1985). Classical conditioning of preferences for stimuli. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, 316–323. doi: 10.1086/208518 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37, 1–20. doi: 10.3758/LB.37.1.1 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. , Thomas, S. , Baeyens, F. (2001). Associative learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 853–869. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.127.6.853 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H. , Eiser, J. R. , Shook, N. J. (2004). Attitude formation through exploration: Valence asymmetries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 293–311. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.293 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H. , Jackson, J. R. , Dunton, B. C. , Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial-attitudes: A bona-fide pipeline. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.6.1013 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Förderer, S. , Unkelbach, C. (2012). Hating the cute kitten or loving the aggressive pit-bull: EC effects depend on CS-US relations. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 534–540. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.588687 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. , Bodenhausen, G. V. (2011). The associative-propositional evaluation model: Theory, evidence, and open questions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 59–127. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-385522-0.00002-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gast, A. , De Houwer, J. , De Schryver, M. (2012). Evaluative conditioning can be modulated by memory of the CS-US pairings at the time of testing. Learning and Motivation, 43, 116–126. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2012.06.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goldstone, R. L. , Steyvers, M. (2001). The sensitization and differentiation of dimensions during category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 116–139. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.1.116 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human memory. Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation, 16, 96–101. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1986). “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 93, 411–428. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.93.4.411 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, 95, 528–551. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.528 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofmann, W. , De Houwer, J. , Perugini, M. , Baeyens, F. , Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 390–421. doi: 10.1037/a0018916 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Huetter, M. , Sweldens, S. , Stahl, C. , Unkelbach, C. , Klauer, K. C. (2012). Dissociating contingency awareness and conditioned attitudes: Evidence of contingency-unaware evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 539–557. doi: 10.1037/a0026477 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Imhoff, R. , Schmidt, A. F. , Bernhardt, J. , Dierksmeier, A. , Banse, R. (2011). An inkblot for sexual preference: A semantic variant of the affect misattribution procedure. Cognition and Emotion, 25, 676–690. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.508260 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kalish, H. (1969). Stimulus generalization. In M. Marx (Ed.), Learning: Processes (pp. 205–297). Oxford, UK: Macmillan. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kenward, M. G. , Roger, J. H. (1997). Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood. Biometrics, 53, 983–997. doi: 10.2307/2533558 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K. C. (2009). Spontaneous evaluations. In F. Strack, J. Förster (Eds.), Social cognition: The basis of human interaction (pp. 199–217). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lamberts, K. (2000). Information-accumulation theory of speeded categorization. Psychological Review, 107, 227–260. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.107.2.227 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J. , Bradley, M. M. , Cuthbert, B. N. (2001). International affective picture system (IAPS): Instruction manual and affective ratings. [Technical Report A-5] Gainesville, FL: Center for Research in Physiology, University of Florida First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Le Pelley, M. E. , Calvini, G. , Spears, R. (2013). Learned predictiveness influences automatic evaluations in human contingency learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 217–228. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Le Pelley, M. E. , Reimers, S. J. , Calvini, G. , Spears, R. , Beesley, T. , Murphy, R. A. (2010). Stereotype formation: Biased by association. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 138–161. doi: 10.1037/a0018210 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Le Pelley, M. E. , Suret, M. B. , Beesley, T. (2009). Learned predictiveness effects in humans: A function of learning, performance, or both? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 312–327. doi: 10.1037/a0014315 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lissek, S. , Biggs, A. L. , Rabin, S. J. , Cornwell, B. R. , Alvarez, R. P. , Pine, D. S. , Grillon, C. (2008). Generalization of conditioned fear-potentiated startle in humans: Experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 678–687. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.005 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention. Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Medin, D. L. , Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review, 85, 207–238. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.85.3.207 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mitchell, C. J. , De Houwer, J. , Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 183–246. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X09000855 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nosofsky, R. M. (1984). Choice, similarity, and the context of classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 104–114. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.10.1.104 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nosofsky, R. M. (1986). Attention, similarity, and the identification-categorization relationship. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 39–57. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nosofsky, R. M. (1987). Attention and learning processes in identification and categorization of integral stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 87–108. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.13.1.87 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nosofsky, R. M. , Palmeri, T. J. (1997). An exemplar-based random walk model of speeded classification. Psychological Review, 104, 266–300. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.446 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Olson, M. A. , Fazio, R. H. (2006). Reducing automatically activated racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 421–433. doi: 10.1177/0146167205284004 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. , Brown-Iannuzzi, J. , Burkley, M. , Arbuckle, N. L. , Cooley, E. , Cameron, C. D. , Lundberg, K. B. (2013). Intention invention and the affect misattribution procedure: Reply to Bar-Anan and Nosek (2012). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 375–386. doi: 10.1177/0146167212475225 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. , Cheng, C. M. , Govorun, O. , Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. , Govorun, O. , Arbuckle, N. L. (2008). Automatic attitudes and alcohol: Does implicit liking predict drinking? Cognition and Emotion, 22, 238–271. doi: 10.1080/02699930701357394 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. , Hall, D. L. , Cameron, C. D. , Bishara, A. J. (2010). A process model of affect misattribution. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1397–1408. doi: 10.1177/0146167210383440 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pleyers, G. , Corneille, O. , Luminet, O. , Yzerbyt, V. (2007). Aware and (dis)liking: Item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 130–144. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.1.130 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Pleyers, G. , Corneille, O. , Yzerbyt, V. , Luminet, O. (2009). Evaluative conditioning may incur attentional costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 279–285. doi: 10.1037/a0013429 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Razran, G. (1949). Stimulus generalization of conditioned responses. Psychological Bulletin, 46, 337–365. doi: 10.1037/h0060507 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rescorla, R. A. (1976). Stimulus generalization: Some predictions from a model of Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 88–96. doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.2.1.88 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Sachs, D. H. (1975). Conditioning of affect to a neutral stimulus: Number of trials. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 40, 895–901. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shook, N. J. , Fazio, R. H. , Eiser, J. R. (2007). Attitude generalization: Similarity, valence, and extremity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 641–647. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp. 2006.06.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, E. R. , Zárate, M. A. (1992). Exemplar-based model of social judgment. Psychological Review, 99, 3–21. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A. , Clarysse, J. , Vansteenwegen, D. , Baeyens, F. , Hermans, D. (2010). Affect 4.0: A free software package for implementing psychological and psychophysiological experiments. Experimental Psychology, 57, 36–45. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000005 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A. , Hermans, D. , De Houwer, J. , Eelen, P. (2002). On the nature of the affective priming effect: Affective priming of naming responses. Social Cognition, 20, 227–256. doi: 10.1521/soco.20.3.227.21106 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spruyt, A. , Hermans, D. , De Houwer, J. , Vandekerckhove, J. , Eelen, P. (2007). On the predictive validity of indirect attitude measures: Prediction of consumer choice behavior on the basis of affective priming in the picture-picture naming task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 599–610. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.06.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Staats, A. W. , Staats, C. K. (1959). Effect of number of trials on the language conditioning of meaning. The Journal of General Psychology, 61, 211–223. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Stahl, C. , Unkelbach, C. , Corneille, O. (2009). On the respective contributions of awareness of unconditioned stimulus valence and unconditioned stimulus identity in attitude formation through evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 404–420. doi: 10.1037/a0016196 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Till, B. D. , Priluck, R. L. (2000). Stimulus generalization in classical conditioning: An initial investigation and extension. Psychology & Marketing, 17, 55–72. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6793(200001)17:1<55::AID-MAR4>3.0.CO;2-C First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unkelbach, C. , Stahl, C. , Förderer, S. (2012). Changing CS features alters evaluative responses in evaluative conditioning. Learning & Motivation, 43, 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2012.04.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vervliet, B. , Iberico, C. , Vervoort, E. , Baeyens, F. (2011). Generalization gradients in human predictive learning: Effects of discrimination training and within-subjects testing. Learning and Motivation, 42, 210–220. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2011.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vervliet, B. , Kindt, M. , Vansteenwegen, D. , Hermans, D. (2010). Fear generalization in humans: Impact of verbal instructions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 38–43. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.09.005 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Vervliet, B. , Vansteenwegen, D. , Eelen, P. (2004). Generalization of extinguished skin conductance responding in human fear conditioning. Learning & Memory, 11, 555–558. doi: 10.1101/lm.77404 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Vervliet, B. , Vansteenwegen, D. , Eelen, P. (2006). Generalization gradients for acquisition and extinction in human contingency learning. Experimental Psychology, 53, 132–142. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.2.132 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Zanon, R. , De Houwer, J. , Gast, A. (2012). Context effects in evaluative conditioning of implicit evaluations. Learning and Motivation, 43, 155–165. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2012.02.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar