Skip to main content
Short Research Article

Stroke Encoding Processes of Chinese Character During Sentence Reading

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000478

Abstract. The present study was conducted to examine whether traditional and simplified Chinese readers (TCRs and SCRs) differed in stroke encoding in character processing by an eye-tracking experiment. We recruited 66 participants (32 TCRs and 34 SCRs) to read sentences comprising characters with different proportions and types of strokes removed in order to explore whether any visual complexity effect existed in their processing of simplified and traditional Chinese characters. The present study found a cross-script visual complexity effect and that SCRs were more influenced by visual complexity change in lexical access than were TCRs. In addition, the stroke-order effect appeared to be more salient for TCRs than for SCRs.

References

  • Abdelhadi, S., Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading of Arabic: Effects of orthographic visual complexity on detection. Writing Systems Research, 3, 117–127. 10.1093/wsr/wsr014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cao, C. Y., & Shen, Y. (1963). Preliminary research about the tachistoscopic identification of Chinese characters of children [in Chinese]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 7, 271–279. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, L., & Wang, L. (2003). Linguistic awareness in learning to read Chinese: A comparative study of Beijing and Hong Kong children. In C. McBride-ChangH.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 91–106). Westport, CT: Praeger. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, X., & Kao, H. S. (2002). Visualspatial properties and orthographic processing of Chinese characters. In H. S. R. KaoC.-K. LeongD.-G. Gao (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience studies of the Chinese language (pp. 175–194). Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, H.-C., Song, H., Lau, W. Y., Wong, K. F. E., & Tang, S. L. (2003). Developmental characteristics of eye movements in reading Chinese. In C. McBride-ChangH.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 159–169). Westport, CT: Praeger. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, M. J., & Yuen, J. C.-K. (1991). Effects of pinyin and script type on verbal processing: Comparisons of China, Taiwan, and Hong Kong experience. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 14, 429–448. 10.1177/016502549101400405 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cheng, C.-M. (1981). Perception of Chinese characters. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 23, 137–153. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cheng, C.-M., & Fu, G.-L. (1986). The recognition of Chinese characters and words under divided visual field presentation. Linguistics, Psychology, and the Chinese Language, 23–37. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dufau, S., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P. (2008). An ERP investigation of location invariance in masked repetition priming. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 222–228. 10.3758/cabn.8.2.222 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Flores d’Arcais, G. B. (1994). Order of strokes writing as a cue for retrieval in reading Chinese characters. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 6, 337–355. 10.1080/09541449408406519 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104–115. 10.1037/0096-1523.13.1.104 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Gao, D.-G., & Kao, H. S. R. (2002). Psycho-geometric analysis of commonly used Chinese characters. In C.-K. L. H. S. R. KaoD.-G. Gao (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience studies of the Chinese language. (pp. 195–206). Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Huang, K.-C., & Hsu, S.-H. (2005). Effects of numbers of strokes on Chinese character recognition during a normal reading condition. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 101, 845–852. 10.2466/pms.101.3.845-852 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Metalinguistic awareness and reading performance: A cross language comparison. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 297–317. 10.1007/s10936-006-9046-3 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1987). Orthography: Its structure and effects on reading. In M. A. JustP. A. Carpenter (Eds.), The psychology of reading and language processing (pp. 287–325). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. Advances in Psychology, 94, 67–84. 10.1016/s0166-4115(08)62789-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Leong, C. K. (1986). What does accessing a morphemic script tell us about reading and reading disorders in an alphabetic script? Annals of Dyslexia, 36, 82–102. 10.1007/bf02648023 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Leong, C. K., Cheng, P.-W., & Mulcahy, R. (1987). Automatic processing of morphemic orthography by mature readers. Language and Speech, 30, 181–196. 10.1177/002383098703000207 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Magnuson, J. S. (2019). Fixations in the visual world paradigm: Where, when, why? Journal of Cultural Cognition, 3, 111139. 10.1007/s41809-019-00035-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McBride-Chang, C., Chow, B. W., Zhong, Y., Burgess, S., & Hayward, W. G. (2005). Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts. Reading and Writing, 18, 99–128. 10.1007/s11145-004-7343-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 940–967. 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067) First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372422. 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Rayner, K. (2009). The 35th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psycholology, 62, 1457–1506. 10.1080/17470210902816461 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Su, P. C. (2001). The 20th century’s study on contemporary Chinese characters contents [in Chinese]. Shanxi: Shuhai Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tan, L. H., & Peng, D. L. (1990). Yujing dui hanyu danzici tezheng fenxi de yingxiang [The effects of semantic context on the feature analyses of single Chinese characters]. Xin li xue dong tai [Journal of Psychology], 4, 5–10. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tsai, J.-L., Kliegl, R., & Yan, M. (2012). Parafoveal semantic information extraction in traditional Chinese reading. Acta Psychologica, 141, 17–23. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.06.004 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Tsai, J.-L., & McConkie, G. W. (2003). Where do Chinese readers send their eyes. In J. HyönäR. RadachH. Deubel (Eds.), The mind’s eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research (pp. 159–176). North Holland: Elsevier. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tseng, S. C., Chang, L., & Hsiang, W. C. C. (1965). An informational analysis of the Chinese language: I. The reconstruction of the removed strokes of the ideograms in printed sentence-texts. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 9, 281–290. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, W. S.-Y. (1973). The Chinese language. Scientific American, 228, 50–60. 10.1038/scientificamerican0273-50 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, W. S.-Y. (1981). Language structure and optimal orthography. In O. J. L. TzengH. Spinger (Eds.), Perception of print: Reading research in experimental psychology (pp. 223–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, H.-C., Pomplun, M., Chen, M., Ko, H., & Rayner, K. (2010). Estimating the effect of word predictability on eye movements in Chinese reading using latent semantic analysis and transitional probability. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1374–1386. 10.1080/17470210903380814 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yan, G. L., Bai, X. J., Zang, C. L., Bian, Q., Cui, L., Qi, W., Rayner, K. & Liversedge, S. P. (2012). Using stroke removal to investigate Chinese character identification during reading: evidence from eye movements. Reading and Writing, 25, 951–979. 10.1007/s11145-011-9295-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yang, H.-M., & McConkie, G. W. (1999). Reading Chinese: Some basic eye-movement characteristics. In J. WangH. C. ChenR. RadachA. Inhoff (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 207–222). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yeh, J. S., & Liu, I. M. (1972). Factors affecting recognition thresholds of Chinese characters. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 14, 113–117. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yip, P. C. (2000). The Chinese lexicon: A comprehensive survey. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yu, B. L., & Cao, H. Q. (1992). The effect of the stroke-number disposition on Chinese character recognition. Psychological Science, 4, 5–10. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zhu, X. (1991). The effect of Chinese sentence context on word recognition. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 23, 145–152. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar