Skip to main content
Original Article

The Police Officer’s Dilemma

The Relationship Between Violent Video Game Play and Responses in a First-Person Shooter Task

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000260

Abstract. Correll and colleagues (Correll, Park, Judd, & Wittenbrink, 2002) developed a first-person shooter task that simulates the police officer’s dilemma of whether to shoot or not a target that may present lethal danger. The present study examined the relationship between habitual violent video game play and responses in this shooting paradigm. Habitual violent video game play has been shown to increase the accessibility of aggressive thoughts. Previous research also demonstrated that action video game play has a positive impact on perceptual skills. Hence, it was hypothesized that players of violent video games would be more likely to mistakenly shoot a target and exhibit shorter reaction times in the shooting task. Results revealed that reaction times, but not error rates, were significantly associated with habitual violent video game play. These findings suggest that habitual violent video game play may have a positive impact on overall processing skills without limiting accuracy.

References

  • Anderson, C. A., Carnagey, N. L., Flanagan, M., Benjamin, A. J., Eubanks, J., & Valentine, J. C. (2004). Violent video games: Specific effects of violent content on aggressive thoughts and behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 199–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(04)36004-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., … Saleem, A. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018251 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barlett, C., Branch, O., Rodeheffer, C., & Harris, R. (2009). How long do the short-term violent video game effects last? Aggressive Behavior, 35, 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20301 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bartholow, B. D., Bushman, B. J., & Sestir, M. A. (2006). Chronic violent video game exposure and desensitization to violence: Behavioral and event-related brain potential data. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 532–539. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bavelier, D., Green, C. S., Pouget, A., & Schrater, P. (2012). Brain plasticity through the life span: Learning to learn and action video games. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 391–416. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-152832 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bediou, B., Adams, D. M., Mayer, R. E., Tipton, E., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2018). Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychological Bulletin, 144, 77–110. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000130 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-Aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59–73. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bryant, F. B., & Smith, B. D. (2001). Refining the architecture of aggression: A measurement model for the Buss-Perry aggression questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 138–167. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.2000.2302 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bushman, B. J. (1996). Individual differences in the extent and development of aggressive cognitive–associative networks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 811–819. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167296228004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2002). Violent video games and hostile expectations: A test of the general aggression model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1679–1686. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616702237649 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Correll, J., Hudson, S. M., Guillermo, S., & Ma, D. S. (2014). The police officer’s dilemma: A decade of research on racial bias in the decision to shoot. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 201–213. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12099 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2002). The police officer’s dilemma: Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1314–1329. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Correll, J., Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2007). The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 1102–1117. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.450 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dobrowolski, P., Hanusz, K., Sobczyk, B., Skorko, M., & Wiatrow, A. (2015). Cognitive enhancement in video game players: The role of video game genre. Computers in Human Behavior, 44, 59–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.051 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dye, M. W., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2009). Increasing speed of processing with action video games. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 321–326. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01660.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Engelhardt, C. R., Mazurek, M. O., Hilgard, J., Rouder, J. N., & Bartholow, B. D. (2015). Effects of violent-video-game exposure on aggressive behavior, aggressive-thought accessibility, and aggressive affect among adults with and without autism spectrum disorder. Psychological Science, 26, 1187–1200. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615583038 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Essien, I., Stelter, M., Kalbe, F., Koehler, A., Mangels, J., & Meliss, S. (2017). The shooter bias: Replicating the classic effect and introducing a novel paradigm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 70, 41–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.12.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gentile, D. G., Li, D., Khoo, A., Prot, S., & Anderson, C. A. (2014). Mediators and moderators of long-term violent video game effects on aggressive behavior: Practice, thinking, and action. JAMA Pediatrics, 168, 450–457. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.63 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gonzalez, J. M., & Greitemeyer, T. (2018). The relationship between everyday sadism, violent video game play, and fascination with weapons. Personality and Individual Differences, 124, 51–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.049 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greitemeyer, T. (2014). Intense acts of violence during video game play make daily life aggression appear innocuous: A new mechanism why violent video games increase aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 52–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.09.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greitemeyer, T., & Mügge, D. O. (2014). Video games do affect social outcomes: A meta-analytic review of the effects of violent and prosocial video game play. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 578–589. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213520459 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grizzard, M., Tamborini, R., Sherry, J. L., Weber, R., Prabhu, S., Hahn, L., & Idzik, P. (2015). The thrill is gone, but you might not know: Habituation and generalization of biophysiological and self-reported arousal responses to video games. Communication Monographs, 82, 64–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751.2014.971418 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21, 28–41. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kirsh, S. J. (1998). Seeing the world through Mortal Kombat-colored glasses: Violent video games and the development of a short-term hostile attribution bias. Childhood, 5, 177–184. https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568298005002005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kirsh, S. J., Olczak, P. V., & Mounts, J. R. (2005). Violent video games induce an affect processing bias. Media Psychology, 7, 239–250. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532785XMEP0703_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCarthy, R. J., Coley, S. L., Wagner, M. F., Zengel, B., & Basham, A. (2016). Does playing video games with violent content temporarily increase aggressive inclinations? A pre-registered experimental study. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 13–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mekawi, Y., & Bresin, K. (2015). Is the evidence from racial bias shooting task studies a smoking gun? Results from a meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 61, 120–130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.08.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oei, A. C., & Patterson, M. D. (2014). Are videogame training gains specific or general? Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 54. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00054 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.488 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Plouffe, R. A., Saklofske, D. H., & Smith, M. M. (2017). The assessment of sadistic personality: Preliminary psychometric evidence for a new measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 166–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.043 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1660–1672. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1660 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tamborini, R., Eastin, M. S., Skalski, P., Lachlan, K., Fediuk, T. A., & Brady, R. (2004). Violent virtual video games and hostile thoughts. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 48, 335–357. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Unkelbach, C., Forgas, J. P., & Denson, T. F. (2008). The turban effect: The influence of Muslim headgear and induced affect on aggressive responses in the shooter bias paradigm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1409–1413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar