Skip to main content
Original Article

Fast and Not Furious?

Reduction of Cognitive Dissonance in Smokers

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000086

Three studies explored whether cognitive dissonance in smokers is reduced immediately or remains constant due to the perceived health risk. Because dissonance-reducing strategies might occur very quickly and previous research has focused only on ratings concerning health risk, we additionally analyzed response latencies and psychophysiological arousal as more implicit measurements. In Study 1, 2, and 3, participants rated their smoking-related health risks twice for different diseases. Ratings, response latencies (Study 1, 2), and psychophysiological arousal (Study 3) differed during the first testing. Differences in response latencies and psychophysiological arousal diminished during the second testing, whereas ratings did not change. The results are discussed in terms of implicit methods as measurements for cognitive dissonance and in terms of prevention and intervention programs.

References

  • Arcuri, L. , Castelli, L. , Boca, S. , Lorenzi-Cioldi, F. , Dafflon, A. (2001). Fuzzy gender categories: How emotional expression influences typicality. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 60, 179–191. doi: 10.1024//1421-0185.60.3.179 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Aronson, E. (1969). The theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perspective. In L. Berkowitz, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–34). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Aronson, E. (1992). The return of the repressed: Dissonance theory makes a comeback. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 303–311. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0304_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Backman, C. W. (1988). The self: A dialectical approach. In L. Berkowitz, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 229–260). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman, J. A. Bargh, (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beauvois, J.-L. , Joule, R. V. (1996). A radical dissonance theory. London: Taylor & Francis. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beauvois, J.-L. , Joule, R. V. , Brunetti, F. (1993). Cognitive rationalization and act rationalization in escalation of commitment. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 14, 1–17. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp1401_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bolls, P. D. , Lang, A. , Potter, R. F. (2001). The effects of message valence and listener arousal on attention, memory and facial muscular responses to radio. Communication Research, 28, 627–651. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cook, D. , Pallak, M. , Storms, M. , McCaul, K. (1977). The effect of forced compliance on attitude change and behavior change. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 71–74. doi: 10.1177/014616727600300110 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cooper, J. (1992). Dissonance and the return of the self-concept. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 320–323. doi: 10.1207/s15327965 pli0304_5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cooper, J. , Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance theory. In L. Berkowitz, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 229–260). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Croyle, R. T. , Cooper, J. (1983). Dissonance arousal: Physiological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 782–791. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.45.4.782 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dawson, M. E. , Schell, A. M. , Filion, D. L. (2000). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, G. G. Berntson, (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 200–223). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Elkin, R. , Leippe, M. (1986). Physiological arousal, dissonance, and attitude change: Evidence for a dissonance-arousal link and a “don’t remind me” effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 55–65. doi: 10.1037//0022–3514.51.1.55 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. , Devine, P. G. (1994). On the motivational nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 382–394. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.67.3.382 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Engle, R. W. , Tuholski, S. W. , Laughlin, J. E. , Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence. A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology: General, 128, 309–331. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.128.3.309 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fazio, R. H. (1990). A practical guide to the use of response latencies in social psychological research. In C. Hendricks, M. S. Clark, (Eds.), Research methods in personality and social psychology (pp. 74–97). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Festinger, L. , Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 58, 203–210. doi: 10.1037/h0041593 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gibbons, F. X. , Eggleston, T. J. , Benthin, A. C. (1997). Cognitive reactions to smoking relapse: The reciprocal relation between dissonance and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 184–195. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514. 72.1.184 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gibbons, F. X. , Gerrard, M. , Lando, H. A. , McGovern, P. G. (1991). Social comparison and smoking cessation: The role of the typical smoker. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27, 239–258. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(91)90014-W First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gibbons, F. X. , McGovern, P. G. , Lando, H. A. (1991). Relapse and risk perception among members of a smoking cessation clinic. Health Psychology, 10, 42–45. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.10.1.42 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Glock, S. (2010). Rauchen ist uncool! Die Repräsentation der verschiedenen Aspekte des Rauchens im Selbstkonzept und ihre Bedeutung für die Veränderung des Verhaltens [Smoking is uncool! The smoking self-concept and the impact of different smoking aspects on behavior change]. Hamburg: Dr. Kovac. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gosling, P. , Denizeau, M. , Oberlé, D. (2006). Denial of responsibility: A new mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 722–733. doi: 10.1037/ 0022-3514.90.5.722 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hemsley, G. D. , Marmurek, H. H. C. (1982). Person memory: The role of consistent and inconsistent person information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 433–438. doi: 10.1177/0146167282083007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hendricks, P. S. , Brandon, T. H. (2005). Smoking expectancy associates among college smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 30, 235–245. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.05.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopkins, R. , Fletcher, J. E. (1994). Electrodermal measurement: Particularly effective for forecasting message influence on sales appeal. In A. Lang, (Ed.), Measuring psychological responses to media messages (pp. 113–132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, E. E. (1976). Forward. In R. A. Wicklund, J. W. Brehm, (Eds.), Explorations in cognitive dissonance (pp. x–xii). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Klesges, R. C. , Somes, G. , Pascale, R. W. , Klesges, L. M. , Murphy, M. , Brown, K. , Williams, E. (1988). Knowledge and beliefs regarding the consequences of cigarette smoking and their relationships to smoking status in a biracial sample. Health Psychology, 7, 384–401. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.7.5.387 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kneer, J. , Glock, S. (2008). Werden Raucher von Warnhinweisen auf Zigarettenschachteln beeinflusst? Eine sozialpsychologische Perspektive. [Is there any impact of cigarette warning labels on smokers? A perspective from social psychology]. Zeitschrift für Sozialmanagement, 6, 99–108. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lando, H. A. (1989). Treatment outcome evaluation methodology in smoking cessation: Strengths and key issues. Advances in Behavior Research Therapy, 11, 201–214. doi: 10.1016/0146-6402(89)90023-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, A. (2006). Using the limited capacity model of motivated mediated message processing to design effective cancer communication messages. Journal of Communication, 56, 57–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2006.00283.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lee, C. (1988). Perceptions of immunity to disease in adult smokers. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12, 267–277. doi: 10.1007/BF00844871 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lieberman, M. D. , Ochsner, K. N. , Gilbert, D. T. , Schacter, D. L. (2001). Do amnesics exhibit cognitive dissonance reduction? The role of explicit memory and attention in attitude change. Psychological Science, 12, 135–140. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280. 00323 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.95.4.492 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Losch, M. E. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1990). Cognitive dissonance may enhance sympathetic tonus, but attitudes are changed to reduce negative affect rather than arousal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 289–304. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031 (90) 90040-S First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Macrae, C. N. , Bodenhausen, G. V. , Milne, A. B. (1994). Out of mind, back in sight: Stereotypes on the rebound. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 808–817. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.5.808 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mann, L. , Janis, I. , Chaplin, R. (1969). Effects of an anticipation of forthcoming information on predecisional processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11, 10–16. doi: 10.1037/h0026967 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martinie, M.-A. , Olive, T. , Milland, L. (2010). Cognitive dissonance induced by writing a counterattitudinal essay facilitates performance on simple tasks but not on complex tasks that involve working memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 587–594. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.10.018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Monteith, M. J. , Sherman, J. W. , Devine, P. G. (1998). Suppression as a stereotype control strategy. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 63–82. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0201_4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Monteith, M. J. , Spicer, C. V. , Tooman, G. D. (1998). Consequences of stereotype suppression: Stereotypes on and not on the rebound. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 355–377. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1998.1355 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mullenix, J. W. , Kilbey, M. M. , Fisicaro, S. A. , Farnsworth, L. M. , Torrento, R. (2003). The representation of smoking expectancies by single-word adjectives and the relationship to smoking characteristics. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 5, 682–694. doi: 10.1080/1462220031000158654 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Newby-Clark, I. R. , McGregor, I. , Zanna, M. P. (2002). Thinking and caring about cognitive inconsistency: When and for whom does attitudinal ambivalence feel uncomfortable? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 157–166. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.82.2.157-166 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pallak, M. , Brock, T. , Kiesler, C. (1967). Dissonance arousal and task performance in an incidental verbal learning paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 11–20. doi: 10.1037/h0024894 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pallak, M. , Kiesler, C. (1968). Dissonance arousal, task evaluation and task performance. Psychonomic Science, 11, 197–198. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pallak, M. , Pittman, T. (1972). General motivational effects of dissonance arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21, 349–358. doi: 10.1037/h0032266 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). New York: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Plant, E. A. , Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 811–832. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.3.811 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ravaja, N. (2004). Contributions of psychophysiology to media research: Review and recommendations. Media Psychology, 6, 193–235. doi: 10.1207/s1532785xmep0602_4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosen, V. , Engle, R. W. (1998). Working memory capacity and suppression. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 418–436. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2590 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salkovskis, P. M. , Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 32, 193–201. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90112-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shallice, T. (1972). Dual functions of consciousness. Psychological Review, 79, 383–393. doi: 10.1037/h0033135 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sherman, D. A. K. , Nelson, L. D. , Steele, C. M. (2000). Do messages about health risks threaten the self? Increasing the acceptance of threatening health messages via self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1046–1058. doi: 10.1177/01461672002611003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sherman, J. W. , Lee, A. Y. , Bessenoff, G. R. , Frost, L. A. (1998). Stereotype efficiency reconsidered: Encoding flexibility under cognitive load. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 589–606. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.3.589 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sherman, S. J. , Rose, J. S. , Koch, K. , Presson, C. C. , Chassin, L. (2003). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward cigarette smoking: The effects of context and motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 13–39. doi: 10.1521/ jscp.22.1.13.22766 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simmons, V. N. , Webb, M. S. , Brandon, T. H. (2004). College-student smoking: An initial test of an experimental dissonance-enhancing intervention. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 1129–1136. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.03.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simon, L. , Greenberg, G. , Brehm, J. (1995). Trivialization: The forgotten mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 247–260. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.68.2.247 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Statistisches Bundesamt . (2010). Pressemitteilung Nr. 190 vom 28.05.2010: Drei Viertel der Bevölkerung sind Nichtraucher [Press release No. 190, May, 28, 2010: Three quarters of the German Population are nonsmokers]. Retrieved from www.destatis.de/jetspeed/portal/cms/Sites/destatis/Internet/DE/Presse/pm/2010/05/PD10__190__239,templateId=renderPrint.psml First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 261–302). New-York: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stone, J. , Cooper, J. (2001). A self-standards model of cognitive dissonance. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 37, 228–243. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2000.1446 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tedeschi, J. T. , Schlenker, B. R. , Bonoma, T. V. (1971). Cognitive dissonance: Private ratiocination or public spectacle? American Psychologist, 26, 685–695. doi: 10.1037/h0032110 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Harreveld, F. , Van der Pligt, J. , De Vries, N. K. (1999). Attitudes toward smoking and the subjective importance of attributes: Implications for changing risk-benefit ratios. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 58, 65–72. doi: 10.1024//1421-0185.58.2.65 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Visser, P. S. , Cooper, J. (2003). Attitude change. In M. A. Hogg, J. Cooper, (Eds.), The Sage handbook of social psychology (pp. 211–231). London: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Waterman, C. K. (1969). The facilitating and interfering effects of cognitive dissonance on simple and complex paired-associate learning tasks. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 5, 31–42. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(69)90004-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52. doi: 10.1037//0033-295X.101. 1.34 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegner, D. M. , Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 903–912. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.63.6.903 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wenzlaff, R. M. , Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 59–91. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.59 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar