Skip to main content
Replication

Black and White as Valence Cues

A Large-Scale Replication Effort of Meier, Robinson, and Clore (2004)

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000236

Replication efforts involving large samples are recommended in helping to determine the reliability of an effect. This approach was taken for a study from Meier, Robinson, and Clore (2004), one of the first papers in social cognition guided by conceptual metaphor theory, which reported that evaluations were faster when word valence metaphorically matched (e.g., a word with a negative meaning in black) rather than mismatched (e.g., a word with a negative meaning in white) font color. The present investigation was a direct large-scale replication attempt involving 980 participants who completed an experiment using web-based software and were diverse in terms of race, age, and geographical location. Words with a positive meaning were evaluated faster when font color was white rather than black and words with a negative meaning were evaluated faster when font color was black rather than white, replicating the main results of Meier et al. (2004).

References

  • Begley, C. G., & Ellis, L. M. (2012). Raise standards for preclinical cancer research. Nature, 483, 531–533. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bower, B. (2012). The hot and cold of priming: Psychologists are divided on whether unnoticed cues can influence behavior. Science News, 181, 26–29. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brandt, M. J., IJzerman, H., Dijksterhuis, A., Farach, F. J., Geller, J., Giner-Sorolla, R., … van’t Veer, A. (2014). The Replication recipe: What makes for a convincing replication? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 217–224. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chiou, W. B., & Cheng, Y. Y. (2013). In broad daylight, we trust in God! Brightness, the salience of morality, and ethical behavior. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 36, 37–42. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C. L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind, but whose mind? PLoS One, 7, e29081 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029081. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published findings are false. PLoS Med, 2, e124 doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kiefer, M., & Barsalou, L. W. (2013). Grounding the human conceptual system in perception, action, and internal states. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig, (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 381–407). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koole, S. L., & Lakens, D. (2012). Rewarding replications: A sure and simple way to improve psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 608–614. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lakens, D. (2012). Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: Structural overlap predicts categorization times for bipolar concepts presented in vertical space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 726–736. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenges to western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Landau, M. J., Meier, B. P., & Keefer, L. A. (2010). A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1045–1067. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Landau, M. J., Robinson, M. D., & Meier, B. P. (2014). The power of metaphor: Examining its influence on social life. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 82–87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pashler, H., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2012). Editors’ introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: A crisis of confidence? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 528–530. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Peña, J., & Yoo, S. C. (2014). Under pressure: Avatar appearance and cognitive load effects on attitudes, trustworthiness, bidding, and interpersonal distance in a virtual store. Presence, 23, 18–32. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robinson, M. D. (2007). Lives lived in milliseconds: Using cognitive methods in personality research. In R. W. Robbins, R. C. Fraley, & R. Krueger, (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 345–359). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schubert, T. W. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1–21. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schubert, T. W., Murteira, C., Collins, E. C., & Lopes, D. (2013). ScriptingRT: A software library for collecting response latencies in online studies of cognition. PLoS One, 8, e67769 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067769. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. (2013). Socially situated cognition in perspective. Social Cognition, 31, 125–146. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sherman, G. D., & Clore, G. L. (2009). The color of sin: White and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20, 1019–1025. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 11, 1359–1366. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unkelbach, C., Fiedler, K., Bayer, M., Stegmüller, M., & Danner, D. (2008). Why positive information is processed faster: The density hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 36–49. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Unkelbach, C., von Hippel, W., Forgas, J. P., Robinson, M. D., Shakarchi, R. J., & Hawkins, C. (2010). Good things come easy: Subjective exposure frequency and the faster processing of positive information. Social Cognition, 28, 538–555. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webster, G. D., Urland, G. R., & Correll, J. (2012). Can uniform color “color” aggression? Quasi-experimental evidence from professional ice hockey. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, 274–281. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar