No Evidence for Ideological Asymmetry in Dissonance Avoidance
Unsuccessful Close and Conceptual Replications of Nam, Jost, and van Bavel (2013)
Abstract
Abstract.Nam, Jost, and van Bavel (2013) found that conservatives were more likely than liberals to avoid dissonance-arousing situations (viz., writing counter-attitudinal essays in a high-choice situation). A close replication of this original research was unsuccessful, as both liberals and conservatives avoided writing counter-attitudinal essays to similar degrees. We conducted an additional experiment that aimed to conceptually replicate Nam et al. (2013), and to examine whether people whose ideology is threatened might be more likely to avoid dissonance-arousing situations. Again, liberals and conservatives were equally likely to avoid writing counter-attitudinal essays. Threat had no effect on these decisions. A meta-analysis of Nam et al.’s (2013) two studies, the two studies presently reported, and a third supplemental study provide no evidence for asymmetry in dissonance avoidance.
References
1998). The other “authoritarian personality”. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 47–92. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60382-2
(2015). Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook. Science, 348, 1130–1132. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa1160
(2015). Tweeting from left to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychological Science, 26, 1531–1542. doi: 10.1177/0956797615594620
(2015). Bounded openness: The effect of openness to experience on intolerance is moderated by target group conventionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 549–568. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000055
(2014). Liberals and conservatives can show similarities in negativity bias. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 307–308. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13002513
(2011). Elite influence on public opinion in an informed electorate. American Political Science Review, 105, 496–515. doi: 10.1017/S0003055411000165
(2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29, 807–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00668.x
(2015). Liberal bias and the five-factor model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e139. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14001174
(2002). The relationship between need for cognitive closure and political orientation: The mediating role of authoritarianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 603–610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00668.x
(1995). Preference for consistency: The development of a valid measure and the discovery of surprising behavioral implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 318–328.
(2016). Are conservatives really more simple-minded than liberals? The domain specificity of complex thinking. Political Psychology, 37, 777–798. doi: 10.1111/pops.12304
(2012). The political left rolls with the good and the political right confronts the bad: Connecting physiology and cognition to preferences. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367, 640–649. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0268
(2007). Expertise, evaluative motivation, and the structure of citizens’ ideological commitments. Political Psychology, 28, 535–561. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2007.00589.x
(1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
(1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203–210. doi: 10.1037/h0041593
(2014). Political conservatives’ affinity for obedience to authority is loyal, not blind. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 1205–1214. doi: 10.1177/0146167214538672
(2009). Politically motivated reinforcement seeking: Reframing the selective exposure debate. Journal of Communication, 59, 676–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2009.01452.x
(2012). Politicization of science in the public sphere: A study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. American Sociological Review, 77, 167–187. doi: 10.1177/0146167214538672
(2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1
(2011). The contemporary American conservative movement. Annual Review of Sociology, 37, 325–354. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-081309-150050
(1995). Status, ideology, and integrative complexity on the US Supreme Court: Rethinking the politics of political decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 5–20. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.5
(1989). Political bias in the Dogmatism Scale: An item analysis. The Journal of Social Psychology, 129, 117–118. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1989.9711707
(2007). Cognitive dissonance theory after 50 years of development. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 38, 7–16. doi: 10.1024/0044-3514.38.1.7
(2013). Predisposed: Liberals the biology of political differences. New York, NY: Routledge.
(2014). Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 297–307. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X13001192
(2000). Please, let’s get an answer – any answer: Need for consumer cognitive closure. Psychology & Marketing, 17, 911–934. doi: 10.1002/1520-6793(200011)17:11
(2012). Political diversity in social and personality psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 496–503. doi: 10.1177/1745691612448792
(2008). Selective exposure to campaign communication: The role of anticipated agreement and issue public membership. Journal of Politics, 70, 186–200. doi: 10.1017/S0022381607080139
(1996). The need to evaluate. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 172–194. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.172
(2009). Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 307–337. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163600
(2013).
(Political ideologies and their social psychological functions . In M. FreedonL. T. SargentM. StearsEds., The Oxford handbook of political ideologies (pp. 232–250). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
(2009). Can a psychological theory of ideological differences explain contextual variability in the contents of political attitudes? Psychological Inquiry, 20, 183–188. doi: 10.1080/10478400903088908
(2012, February 14). Mooney’s revenge?! Is there “asymmetry” in Motivated Numeracy? Retrieved from http://www.culturalcognition.net/blog/2013/10/10/mooneys-revenge-is-there-asymmetry-in-motivated-numeracy.html.
(2013). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection: An experimental study. Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 407–424. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.2182588
(2010). The effects of mortality salience on political preferences: The roles of charisma and political orientation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 139–145. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.09.002
(2005). Threat, authoritarianism, and selective exposure to information. Political Psychology, 26, 219–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2005.00416.x
(2009). Citizens’ perceptions of ideological bias in research on public policy controversies. Political Psychology, 30, 43–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00680.x
(2014). Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 1031–1051. doi: 10.1037/a0036170
(2013). Political differences in past, present, and future life satisfaction: republicans are more sensitive than democrats to political climate. PloS One, 9, e98854. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098854
(2013). “Not for all the tea in China!” Political ideology and the avoidance of dissonance-arousing situations. PloS One, 8, e59837. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059837
(2010). When corrections fail: The persistence of political misperceptions. Political Behavior, 32, 303–330. doi: 10.1007/s11109-010-9112-2
(2008). Political attitudes vary with physiological traits. Science, 321, 1667–1670. doi: 10.1126/science.1157627
(1972). Political bias in the Rokeach dogmatism scale. Psychological Reports, 30, 805–806. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1972.30.3.805
(2013). Motivated reasoning and political parties: Evidence for increased processing in the face of party cues. Political Behavior, 35, 831–854. doi: 10.1007/s11109-012-9213-1
(2014, June 26). Beyond red vs. blue: The political typology. Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/files/2014/06/6-26-14-Political-Typology-release.pdf
. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001
(2012). Working toward the experimenter reconceptualizing obedience within the Milgram paradigm as identification-based followership. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 315–324. doi: 10.1177/1745691612448482
(1967). Selective exposure to information: A critical review. Public Opinion Quarterly, 31, 194–213. doi: 10.1086/267513
(2008). Personality and prejudice: A meta-analysis and theoretical review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 248–279. doi: 10.1177/1088868308319226
(2012). Personality and political orientation: Meta-analysis and test of a Threat-Constraint Model. Journal of Research in Personality, 46, 664–677. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2012.08.002
(2015). Small telescopes: Detectability and the evaluation of replication results. Psychological Science, 26, 559–569. doi: 10.1177/0956797614567341
(2015). Extremism reduces conflict arousal and increases values affirmation in response to meaning violations. Biological Psychology, 108, 126–131. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.012
(2011). Linking genetics and political attitudes: Reconceptualizing political ideology. Political Psychology, 32, 369–397. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00821.x
(2006). Motivated skepticism in the evaluation of political beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755–769. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2006.00214.x
(2012). The aftermath of destruction: Images of destroyed buildings increase support for war, dogmatism, and death thought accessibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.05.004
(2000). The relationship between openness to experience and political ideology. Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 741–751. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00135-X
(2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03
(1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049
(1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 264–269. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1968.tb00568.x
(