Skip to main content
Original Article

Strategic Control Over the Unhelpful Effects of Primed Social Categories and Goals

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000112

Social situations can, at times, have a detrimental influence on behavior (e.g., exposure to supermodels can make people dumb, certain social situations can prompt excessive alcohol consumption). Gaining control over such effects can be difficult because the situational influence often occurs outside conscious awareness. The present research investigates whether forming if-then plans or “implementation intentions” (Gollwitzer, 1999) can help people to strategically prevent unwanted social influences. Two experiments found that priming social categories (e.g., supermodels, Experiment 1) or social goals (e.g., socializing, Experiment 2) can have a detrimental impact on participants’ general knowledge (Experiment 1) and mental readiness to drink (Experiment 2), respectively. However, both experiments also showed that forming implementation intentions designed to ensure the effective mobilization of resources (Experiment 1) or to ignore temptation (Experiment 2) prevented social primes from influencing behavior. These findings suggest that if-then planning affords strategic control over unwanted social influences on action.

References

  • Aarts, H. , Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53–63. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.53 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aarts, H. , Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). The silence of the library: Environment, situational norm, and social behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 18–28. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.1.18 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aarts, H. , Dijksterhuis, A. , Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance of mundane behaviours. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 971–979. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199912)29:8<971::AID-EJSP963>3.0.CO;2-A First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J. A. (2006). Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes. New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J. A. , Chartrand, T. L. (2000). Studying the mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. Reis, C. Judd, (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychology (pp. 253–285). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J. A. , Gollwitzer, P. M. , Lee-Chai, A. , Barndollar, K. , Trötschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.6.1014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bargh, J. A. , Raymond, P. , Pryor, J. B. , Strack, F. (1995). Attractiveness of the underling: An automatic power-sex association and its consequences for sexual harassment and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 768–781. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bayer, U. C. , Achtziger, A. , Gollwitzer, P. M. , Moskowitz, G. (2009). Responding to subliminal cues: Do if-then plans facilitate action preparation and initiation without conscious intent?. Social Cognition, 27, 183–201. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brandstätter, V. , Lengfelder, A. , Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.946 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S. , Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for personality, social, clinical and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111–135. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chartrand, T. L. , Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression formation and memorization goals: Nonconscious goal priming reproduces effects of explicit task instructions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 464–478. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijksterhuis, A. , Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un)consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 467–490. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100445 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijksterhuis, A. , Bargh, J. A. (2001). The perception-behaviour expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behaviour. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 1–40. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijksterhuis, A. , Spears, R. , Postmes, T. , Stapel, D. A. , Koomen, W. , van Knippenberg, A. , Sheepers, D. (1998). Seeing one thing and doing another: Contrast effects in automatic behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 862–871. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijksterhuis, A. , van Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between perception and behavior or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 865–877. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. , Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.186 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. , Schaal, B. (1998). Meta-cognition in action: The importance of implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 124–136. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0202_5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. , Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 69–120. doi: 10.1016/50065-2601(06)38002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. , Sheeran, P. , Trötschel, R. , Webb, T. L. (2011). Self-regulation of priming effects on behavior. Psychological Science, 22, 901–907. doi: 10.1177/0956797611411586 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haddock, G. , Macrae, C. N. , Fleck, S. (2003). Syrian science and smart supermodels: On the when and how of perception-behavior effects. Social Cognition, 20, 461–482. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harris, J. L. , Bargh, J. A. , Brownell, K. D. (2009). Priming effects of television food advertising on eating behavior. Health Psychology, 28, 404–413. doi: 10.1037/a0014399 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holland, R. , Aarts, H. , Langendam, D. (2006). Breaking and creating habits on the working floor: A field experiment on the power of implementation intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 776–783. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.11.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hommel, B. , Müsseler, J. , Aschersleben, G. , Prinz, W. (2001). The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lengfelder, A. , Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflexive action control in frontal lobe patients. Neuropsychology, 15, 80–100. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.15.1.80 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Macrae, C. N. , Johnston, L. (1998). Help, I need somebody: Automatic action and inaction. Social Cognition, 16, 400–417. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Malamuth, N. M. (1989). The attraction to sexual aggression scale: Part one. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 26–49. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Malamuth, N. M. (1989). The attraction to sexual aggression scale: Part two. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 324–254. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oettingen, G. , Grant, H. , Smith, P. K. , Gollwitzer, P. M. (2006). Nonconscious goal pursuit: Acting in an explanatory vacuum. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 668–675. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.10.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parks-Stamm, E. J. , Gollwitzer, P. M. , Oettingen, G. (2007). Action control by Implementation intentions: Effective cue detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, 25, 248–266. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parks-Stamm, E. J. , Oettingen, G. , Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Making sense of one’s actions in an explanatory vacuum: The interpretation of nonconscious goal striving. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 531–542. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.02.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sassenberg, K. , Moskowitz, G. B. (2005). Don’t stereotype, think different! Overcoming automatic stereotype activation by mindset priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 506–514. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.10.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheeran, P. , Aarts, H. , Custers, R. , Rivis, A. , Webb, T. L. , Cooke, R. (2005). The goal-dependent automaticity of drinking habits. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 47–64. doi: 10.1348/014466604X23446 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheeran, P. , Webb, T. L. , Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between goal intentions and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271308 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trötschel, R. , Gollwitzer, P. M. (2007). Implementation intentions and the willful pursuit of prosocial goals in negotiations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 579–598. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.06.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Varley, R. , Webb, T. L. , Sheeran, P. (2011). Making self-help more helpful: A randomized controlled trial of the impact of augmenting self-help materials with implementation intentions on promoting the effective self-management of anxiety symptoms. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 123–128. doi: 10.1037/a0021889 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webb, T. L. , Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions overcome ego-depletion?. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 279–286. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00527-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webb, T. L. , Sheeran, P. (2004). Identifying good opportunities to act: Implementation intentions and cue discrimination. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 407–419. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.205 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webb, T. L. , Sheeran, P. (2007). How do implementation intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 295–302. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.02.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webb, T. L. , Sheeran, P. (2008). Mechanisms of implementation intention effects: The role of goal intentions, self-efficacy, and accessibility of plan components. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 373–395. doi: 10.1348/014466607X267010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webb, T. L. , Sheeran, P. , Luszczynska, A. (2009). Planning to break unwanted habits: Habit strength moderates implementation intention effects on behaviour change. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 507–523. doi: 10.1348/014466608X370591 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wechsler, D. (1988). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale third edition (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wieber, F. , Sassenberg, K. (2006). I can’t take my eyes from it – Attention attraction effects of implementation intentions. Social Cognition, 24, 723–752. doi: 10.1521/soco.2006.24.6.723 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, T. D. , Brekke, N. (1984). Mental contamination and mental correction: Unwanted influences on judgments and evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 117–142. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar