Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000171

An important cognitive function is the ability to remember to execute future tasks, a capability known as prospective memory (PM). Workers in complex sociotechnical systems such as healthcare and aviation face many PM challenges and forgetting tasks can have severe consequences. Although researchers have made progress in understanding how individuals remember future tasks, system-level support for PM has seldom been addressed. In the present paper, we briefly review PM research in healthcare and aviation, focusing on naturalistic studies using expert workers, and we present the concept of distributed prospective memory, which incorporates the interaction between the environment and the individual when future tasks must be remembered. PM in sociotechnical settings is a complex process involving human and nonhuman agents. Therefore, a systems approach is needed to fully understand PM processes, thus supporting workers and eventually minimizing errors and increasing safety.

References

  • Altmann, E. M. , Trafton, J. G. (2002). Memory for goals: An activation-based model. Cognitive Science, 26, 39–83. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Button, G. (2008). Against “distributed cognition”. Theory, Culture & Society, 25, 87–104. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Collins, S. A. , Currie, L. , Patel, V. L. , Bakken, S. , & Cimino, J. J. (2007, August). Multitasking by clinicians in the context of CPOE and CIS use. Paper presented at the Congress Proceedings MedInfo 2007, Brisbane, Australia. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dahlbäck, N. , Kristiansson, M. , Stjernberg, F. (2013). Distributed remembering through active structuring of activities and environments. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4, 153–165. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dieckmann, P. , Dyrlov, M. , Reddersen, S. , Rall, M. , Wehner, T. (2009). Remembering to do things later and resuming interrupted tasks: Prospective memory and patient safety. In R. Flin, L. Mitchell, (Eds.), Safer surgery: Analysing behaviour in the operating theatre (pp. 339–352). Farnham, UK: Ashgate. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dieckmann, P. , Reddersen, S. , Wehner, T. , Rall, M. (2006). Prospective memory failures as an unexplored threat to patient safety: Results from a pilot study using patient simulators to investigate the missed execution of intentions. Ergonomics, 49, 526–543. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dismukes, R. K. (2008). Prospective memory in aviation and everyday settings. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, G. O. Einstein, (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 411–431). New York, NY: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dismukes, R. K. (2010). Remembrance of things future: Prospective memory in laboratory, workplace, and everyday settings. Reviews of Human Factors and Ergonomics, 6, 79–122. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dismukes, R. K. , Nowinski, J. (2007). Prospective memory, concurrent task management, and pilot error. In A. Kramer, D. Wiegmann, A. Kirlik, (Eds.), Attention: From theory to practice (pp. 225–236). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dobbs, A. R. , Reeves, B. (1996). Prospective memory: More than memory. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, M. A. McDaniel, (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 199–225). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dodhia, R. M. , Dismukes, R. K. (2009). Interruptions create prospective memory tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 73–89. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Durso, F. T. , Drews, F. A. (2010). Health care, aviation, and ecosystems: A socio-natural systems perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 71–75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ebright, P. R. , Patterson, E. S. , Chalko, B. A. , Render, M. L. (2003). Understanding the complexity of registered nurse work in acute care settings. Journal of Nursing Administration, 33, 630–638. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einstein, G. O. , McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 16, 717–726. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einstein, G. O. , McDaniel, M. A. (2010). Prospective memory and what costs do not reveal about retrieval processes: A commentary on Smith, Hunt, McVay, and McConnell (2007). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1082–1088. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einstein, G. O. , McDaniel, M. A. , Manzi, M. , Cochran, B. , Baker, M. (2000). Prospective memory and aging: Forgetting intentions over short delays. Psychology and Aging, 15, 671–683. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einstein, G. O. , McDaniel, M. A. , Thomas, R. , Mayfield, S. , Shank, H. , Morrisette, N. , Breneiser, J. (2005). Multiple processes in prospective memory retrieval: Factors determining monitoring versus spontaneous retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 327–342. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Einstein, G. O. , McDaniel, M. A. , Williford, C. L. , Pagan, J. L. , Dismukes, R. K. (2003). Forgetting of intentions in demanding situations is rapid. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 147–162. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellis, J. (1996). Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. In M. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, M. A. McDaniel, (Eds.), Prospective memory: Theory and applications (pp. 1–51). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fink, N. , Pak, R. , Bass, B. , Johnston, M. , Battisto, D. (2010, October). A Survey of nurses self-reported prospective memory tasks: What must they remember and what do they forget. Paper presented at the 54th Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, San Francisco, CA. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Flynn, E. A. , Barker, K. N. , Gibson, J. T. , Pearson, R. E. , Berger, B. A. , Smith, L. A. (1999). Impact of interruptions and distractions on dispensing errors in an ambulatory care pharmacy. American Journal of Health-System Pharmacy, 56, 1319–1325. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grundgeiger, T. , Harris, B. , Ford, N. , Sanderson, P. M. , Abbey, M. , Venkatesh, B. (2013). Emergency medical equipment storage: Benefits of visual cues tested in field and simulated settings. Human Factors. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/0018720813514605 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grundgeiger, T. , Liu, D. , Sanderson, P. M. , Jenkins, S. , Leane, T. (2008, September). Effects of interruptions on prospective memory performance in anesthesiology. Paper presented at the 52nd Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, New York, NY. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grundgeiger, T. , Sanderson, P. M. (2009). Interruptions in healthcare: Theoretical views. International Journal of Medical Informatics, 78, 293–307. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grundgeiger, T. , Sanderson, P. M. , Beltran Orihuela, C. , Thompson, A. , MacDougall, H. G. , Nunnink, L. , Venkatesh, B. (2013). Prospective memory in intensive care nursing: A representative and controlled patient simulator study. Ergonomics, 56, 579–589. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grundgeiger, T. , Sanderson, P. M. , MacDougall, H. G. , Venkatesh, B. (2009, October). Distributed prospective memory: An approach to understanding how nurses remember tasks. Paper presented at the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, San Antonio, TX. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grundgeiger, T. , Sanderson, P. M. , MacDougall, H. G. , Venkatesh, B. (2010). Interruption management in the intensive care unit: Predicting resumption times and assessing distributed support. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 317–334. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hersh, N. , & Treadgold, L. (1994). Neuropage: The rehabilitation of memory dysfunction by prosthetic memory and cueing. Neurorehabilitation, 4, 187–197. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hertzog, C. (2008). Commentary: Theories of prospective memory. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, G. O. Einstein, (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 101–114). New York, NY: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Holbrook, J. B. , Dismukes, R. K. (2009, October). Prospective memory in everyday tasks. Paper presented at the 53rd Annual Meeting Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, San Antonio, TX. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hutchins, E. (1995a). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hutchins, E. (1995b). How a cockpit remembers its speeds. Cognitive Science, 19, 265–288. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hutchins, E. (2001). Distributed cognition. In J. S. Neil, B. B. Paul, (Eds.), The International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences (pp. 2068–2072). Oxford, UK: Pergamon. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hutchins, E. (2010). Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 705–715. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kvavilashvili, L. , Fisher, L. (2007). Is time-based prospective remembering mediated by self-initiated rehearsals? Role of incidental cues, ongoing activity, age, and motivation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 112–132. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Liu, D. , Grundgeiger, T. , Sanderson, P. M. , Jenkins, S. , Leane, T. (2009). Interruptions and blood transfusion checks: Lessons from the simulated operating room. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 108, 219–222. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Loft, S. , Humphreys, M. , Neal, A. (2003). Prospective memory in air traffic control. In G. Edkins, P. Pfister, (Eds.), Innovation and consolidation in aviation (pp. 287–293). Aldershot, UK: Ashgate. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Loft, S. , Smith, R. E. , Bhaskara, A. (2011). Prospective memory in an air traffic control simulation: External aids that signal when to act. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17, 60–70. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Loft, S. , Smith, R. E. , Remington, R. W. (2013). Minimizing the disruptive effects of prospective memory in simulated air traffic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 19, 254–265. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Loukopoulos, L. D. , Dismukes, R. K. , Barshi, I. (2009). The multitasking myth: Handling complexity in real-world operations. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Magrabi, F. , Li, S. Y. W. , Day, R. , & Coiera, E. (2010). Errors and electronic prescribing: A controlled laboratory study to examine task complexity and interruption effects. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 17, 575–583. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marsh, R. L. , Hicks, J. L. (1998). Event-based prospective memory and executive control of working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 24, 336–349. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marsh, R. L. , Hicks, J. L. , Cook, G. I. (2006). Task interference from prospective memories covaries with contextual associations of fulfilling them. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1037–1045. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McDaniel, M. A. , Einstein, G. O. (2000). Strategic and automatic processes in prospective memory retrieval: A multiprocess framework. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14, 127–144. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McDaniel, M. A. , Einstein, G. O. , Graham, T. , & Rall, E. (2004). Delaying execution of intentions: Overcoming the costs of interruptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 533–547. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Monk, C. A. , Trafton, J. G. , Boehm-Davis, D. A. (2008). The effect of interruption duration and demand on resuming suspended goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 299–313. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Norman, D. A. (1993). Things that make us smart: Defending human attributes in the age of the machine. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nowinski, J. L. , Dismukes, R. K. (2005). Effects of ongoing task context and target typicality on prospective memory performance: The importance of associative cueing. Memory, 13, 649–657. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nowinski, J. L. , Holbrook, J. B. , Dismukes, R. K. (2003, April). Human memory and cockpit operations: An ASRS study. Paper presented at 12th International Symposium on Aviation Psychology, Dayton, OH. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Parker, J. , & Coiera, E. (2000). Improving clinical communication: A view from psychology. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association, 7, 453–461. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothschild, J. M. , Landrigan, C. P. , Cronin, J. W. , Kaushal, R. , Lockley, S. W. , E., Burdick , … D. W., Bates (2005). The Critical Care Safety Study: The incidence and nature of adverse events and serious medical errors in intensive care. Critical Care Medicine, 33, 1694–1700. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rummel, J. , Boywitt, C. D. , Meiser, T. (2011). Assessing the validity of multinomial models using extraneous variables: An application to prospective memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 2194–2210. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scullin, M. K. , McDaniel, M. A. , Einstein, G. O. (2010). Control of cost in prospective memory: Evidence for spontaneous retrieval processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 36, 190–203. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sellen, A. J. , Louie, G. , Harris, J. E. , Wilkins, A. J. (1997). What brings intentions to mind? An in situ study of prospective memory. Memory, 5, 483–507. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shorrock, S. T. (2005). Errors of memory in air traffic control. Safety Science, 43, 571–588. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, R. E. , Hunt, R. R. , McVay, J. C. , McConnell, M. D. (2007). The cost of event-based prospective memory: Salient target events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 33, 734–746. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, R. E. (2010). What costs do reveal and moving beyond the cost debate: Reply to Einstein and McDaniel (2010). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 36, 1089–1095. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Speier, C. , Vessey, I. , & Valacich, J. S. (2003). The effects of interruptions, task complexity, and information presentation on computer-supported decision-making performance. Decision Sciences, 34, 771–797. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trafton, J. G. , Altmann, E. M. , Brock, D. P. , Mintz, F. E. (2003). Preparing to resume an interrupted task: Effects of prospective goal encoding and retrospective rehearsal. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 58, 583–603. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, M. , Sanderson, P. M. , Russell, W. J. (2004). Tailoring reveals information requirements: The case of anaesthesia alarms. Interacting with Computers, 16, 271–293. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westbrook, J. I. , Woods, A. , Rob, M. I. , Dunsmuir, W. T. M. , Day, R. O. (2010). Association of interruptions with an increased risk and severity of medication administration errors. Archives of Internal Medicine, 170, 683–690. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Xiao, Y. , Milgram, P. , Doyle, D. J. (1997). Planning behavior and its functional role in interactions with complex systems. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics Part A: Systems and Humans, 27, 313–324. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhang, J. J. , Norman, D. A. (1994). Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science, 18, 87–122. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar