Skip to main content
Original Article

Assessing Suffering in Experimental Pain Models

Psychological and Psychophysiological Correlates

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000279

Abstract. Although suffering is a central issue in pain, there is only little research on this topic. The aim of this study was to assess suffering in an experimental context using various stimulation methods and durations, and to examine which psychological or psychophysiological measures covary with pain-related suffering. Twenty-one healthy volunteers participated in two experiments in which we used tonic thermal and phasic electric stimuli with short and long stimulus durations. The participants rated pain intensity, unpleasantness, and pain-related suffering on separate visual analog scales (VAS) and completed the Pictorial Representation of Illness and Self Measure (PRISM), originally developed to assess suffering in chronic illness. We measured heart rate, skin conductance responses (SCRs), and the electromyogram (EMG) of the musculus corrugator supercilii. For both heat and electric pain, we obtained high ratings on the suffering scale confirming that suffering can be evoked in experimental pain conditions. Whereas pain intensity and unpleasantness were highly correlated, both scales were less highly related to suffering, indicating that suffering is distinct from pain intensity and unpleasantness. Higher suffering ratings were associated with more pronounced fear of pain and increased private self-consciousness. Pain-related suffering was also related to high resting heart rate, increased SCR, and decreased EMG during painful stimulation. These results offer an approach to the assessment of suffering in an experimental setting using thermal and electric pain stimulation and shed light on its psychological and psychophysiological correlates.

References

  • Arendt-Nielsen, L., Andersen, O. K. & Jensen, T. S. (1996). Brief, prolonged and repeated stimuli applied to hyperalgesic skin areas: A psychophysiological study. Brain Research, 712, 165–167. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01529-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benedek, M. & Kaernbach, C. (2010a). A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 190, 80–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benedek, M. & Kaernbach, C. (2010b). Decomposition of skin conductance data by means of nonnegative deconvolution. Psychophysiology, 47, 647–658. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00972.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berntson, G. G., Boysen, S. T. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1991). Cardiac orienting and defensive responses: Potential origins in autonomic space. In B. A. CampbellEd., Attention and information processing in infants and adults (pp. 163–200). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bradley, M. M. & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 26, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buchgreitz, L., Egsgaard, L. L., Jensen, R., Arendt-Nielsen, L. & Bendtsen, L. (2008). Abnormal pain processing in chronic tension-type headache: A high-density EEG brain mapping study. Brain Research, 131, 3232–3238. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn199 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buechi, S. & Sensky, T. (1999). PRISM: Pictorial representation of illness and self measure: A brief nonverbal measure of illness impact and therapeutic aid in psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics, 40, 314–320. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(99)71225-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buss, A. (2001). Psychological dimensions of the self. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bustan, S. (in press). The pain and suffering lived matrix. In S. BustanEd., Fundamental transdisciplinary questions on pain and suffering. New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bustan, S., Gonzalez-Roldan, A. M., Kamping, S., Brunner, M., Loeffler, M., Flor, H. & Anton, F. (2015). Suffering as an independent component of the experience of pain. European Journal of Pain, 19, 1035–1048. doi: 10.1002/ejp.709 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cassell, E. J. (1982). The nature of suffering and the goals of medicine. The New England Journal of Medicine, 306, 639–645. doi: 10.1056/NEJM198203183061104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cassell, E. J. (1999). Diagnosing suffering: A perspective. Annals of Internal Medicine, 131, 531–534. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-7-199910050-00009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dirks, J., Petersen, K. & Dahl, J. (2003). The heat/capsaicin sensitization model: A methodological study. The Journal of Pain, 4, 122–128. doi: 10.1054/jpai.2003.10 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ferrell, B. & Coyle, N. (2008). The nature of suffering and the goals of nursing. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fields, H. L. (1999). Pain: An unpleasant topic. Pain, 82, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00139-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Filipp, S. H. & Freudenberg, E. (1989). Fragebogen zur Erfassung dispositionaler Aufmerksamkeit (SAM-Fragebogen) [Questionnaire for the assessment of self-consciousness]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Flor, H., Behle, D. J. & Birbaumer, N. (1993). Assessment of pain-related cognitions in chronic pain patients. Behavior Research and Therapy, 31, 63–73. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90044-U First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fowles, D. C., Christie, M. J., Grings, W. W., Lykken, D. T. & Venables, P. H. (1981). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology, 18, 232–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01384.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fridlund, A. J. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Guidelines for human electromyographic research. Psychophysiology, 23, 567–589. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00676.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Geissner, E. (1996). Schmerzempfindungs-Skala (SES) [Pain Perception Scale]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Glombiewski, J. A., Riecke, J., Holzapfel, S., Rief, W., Konig, S., Lachnit, H. & Seifart, U. (2015). Do patients with chronic pain show autonomic arousal when confronted with feared movements? An experimental investigation of the fear-avoidance model. Pain, 156, 547–554. doi: 10.1097/01.j.pain.0000460329.48633.ce First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hautzinger, M. & Bailer, M. (1993). Allgemeine Depressions Skala [Center of Epidemiological Studies – Depression Scale]. Göttingen, Germany: Beltz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Inui, K. & Kakigi, R. (2012). Pain perception in humans: Use of intraepidermal electrical stimulation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 83, 551–556. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2011-301484 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jennings, J. R., Berg, W. K., Hutcheson, J. S., Obrist, P., Porges, S. & Turpin, G. (1981). Publication guidelines for heart rate studies in man. Psychophysiology, 18, 226–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb03023.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahn, D. L. & Steeves, R. H. (1996). The experience of suffering: Conceptual clarification and theoretical definition. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 11, 623–631. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.1986.tb03379.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kassardjian, C. D., Gardner-Nix, J., Dupak, K., Barbati, J. & Lam-McCullock, J. (2008). Validating PRISM (pictorial representation of illness and self measure) as a measure of suffering in chronic non-cancer pain patients. The Journal of Pain, 9, 1135–1143. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.06.016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krampen, G. (1981). IPC-Fragebogen zur Kontrollüberzeugung [Questionnaire for locus of control]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Krikorian, A., Limonero, J. T. & Corey, M. T. (2013). Suffering assessment: A review of available instruments for use in palliative care. Journal of Palliative Medicine, 16, 130–142. doi: 10.1089/jpm.2012.0370 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krikorian, A., Limonero, J. T., Roman, J. P., Vargas, J. J. & Palacio, C. (2014). Predictors of suffering in advanced cancer. American Journal of Hospice & Palliative Medicine, 31, 534–542. doi: 10.1177/1049909113494092 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krikorian, A., Limonero, J. T., Vargas, J. J. & Palacio, C. (2013). Assessing suffering in advanced cancer patients using pictorial representation of illness and self-measure (PRISM), preliminary validation of the Spanish version in a Latin American population. Support Care Cancer, 21, 3327–3336. doi: 10.1007/s00520-013-1913-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krohne, H. W., Egloff, B., Kohlmann, C. W. & Tausch, A. (1996). Investigations with a German version of the “positive and negative affect schedule” (PANAS). Diagnostica, 42, 139–156. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kröner-Herwig, B., Gaßmann, J., Tromsdorf, M. & Zahrend, E. (2012). The effects of sex and gender role on responses to pressure pain. Psychosocial Medicine, 9, 1–10. doi: 10.3205/psm000079 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laux, L. & Spielberger, C. D. (1981). Das State-Trait-Angstinventar: STAI [State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI)]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lindström, B. R., Mattsson-Marn, I. B., Golkar, A. & Olsson, A. (2013). In your face: Risk of punishment enhances cognitive control and error-related activity in the corrugator supercilii muscle. PLoS One, 8, e65692. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065692 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Loeser, J. D. (1980). Perspectives on pain. In P. TurnerEd., Proceedings of the first world congress on clinical pharmacology and therapeutics (pp. 316–326). London, UK: Macmillan. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Loeser, J. D. & Melzack, R. (1999). Pain: An overview. The Lancet, 353, 1607–1609. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)01311-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Madsen, C. S., Johnsen, B., Fuglsang-Frederiksen, A., Jensen, T. S. & Finnerup, N. B. (2012). Increased contact heat pain and shortened latencies of contact heat evoked potentials following capsaicin-induced heat hyperalgesia. Clinical Neurophysiology, 123, 1429–1436. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.11.032 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCracken, L. M., Faber, S. D. & Janeck, A. S. (1998). Pain-related anxiety predicts non-specific physical complaints in persons with chronic pain. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36, 621–630. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10039-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCracken, L. M., Gross, R. T., Sorg, P. J. & Edmands, T. A. (1993). Prediction of pain in patients with chronic low back pain: Effects of inaccurate prediction and pain-related anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 647–652. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(93)90117-D First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McNeil, D. W., Au, A. R., Zvolensky, M. J., McKee, D. R., Klineberg, I. J. & Ho, C. C. K. (2001). Fear of pain in orofacial pain patients. Pain, 89, 245–252. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00368-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McNeil, D. W. & Rainwater, A. J. (1998). Development of the fear of pain questionnaire-III. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 389–410. doi: 10.1023/A:1018782831217 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Monin, J. K. & Schulz, R. (2009). Interpersonal effects of suffering in older adult caregiving relationships. Psychology and Aging, 24, 681–695. doi: 10.1037/a0016355 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neddermeyer, T. J., Flühr, K. & Lötsch, J. (2008). Principle components analysis of pain thresholds to thermal, electrical, and mechanical stimuli suggests a predominant common source of variance. Pain, 138, 286–291. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.12.015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Osman, A., Breitenstein, J., Barrios, F., Gutierrez, P. & Kopper, B. (2002). The fear of pain questionnaire-III: Further reliability and validity with nonclinical samples. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 155–173. doi: 10.1023/a:1014884704974 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Price, D. D. (2000). Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science, 288, 1769–1772. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stein, C. & Mendl, G. (1988). The German counterpart to McGill pain questionnaire. Pain, 32, 251–255. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(88)90074-7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Streffer, M.-L., Buechi, S., Mörgeli, H., Galli, U. & Ettlin, D. (2009). PRISM (pictorial representation of illness and self measure): A novel visual instrument to assess pain and suffering in orofacial pain patients. Journal of Orofacial Pain, 23, 140–146. doi: 10.5167/uzh-25322 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thayer, J. F. & Lane, R. D. (2009). Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 33, 81–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vlaeyen, J. W. S., Seelen, H. A. M., Peters, M., de Jong, P., Aretz, E., Beisiegel, E. & Weber, W. E. J. (1999). Fear of movement/(re)injury and muscular reactivity in chronic low back pain patients: An experimental investigation. Pain, 82, 297–304. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00054-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weiss, L. (1996). Modulation des Schmerzerlebens und-verhaltens durch Entspannung – Einfluss der Art der Entspannungsinduktion und des Einsatzzeitpunktes der Entspannung [Modulation of pain experience and behavior by relaxation – the influence of relaxation induction type and timing]. Dissertation, Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wittchen, H.-U., Zaudig, M. & Fydrich, T. (1997). SKID-I. Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV [Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar