Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/0044-3514.36.3.143

Zusammenfassung: Die Forschung zur Terror Management Theorie hat gezeigt, dass die experimentelle Induktion der kognitiven Verfügbarkeit eigener Sterblichkeit (“Mortalitätssalienz”) bei Menschen das Bestreben erhöht, das eigene kulturelle Weltbild zu verteidigen. Vor allem in US-amerikanischen Studien hat sich dies in einer erhöhten Verteidigung nationaler Symbole niedergeschlagen. Wir berichten über eine Studie, in der wir den Einfluss von Mortalitätssalienz auf Reaktionen auf die Verteidigung der Geschehnisse um die deutsche Wiedervereinigung und die Einstellung zur DM in Ost- und Westdeutschland untersucht haben. Unsere Ergebnisse zeigen, dass Personen aus Ostdeutschland nach Mortalitätssalienz verglichen mit einer Kontrollbedingung eine verstärkte Tendenz aufwiesen, die deutsche Einheit gegenüber Kritik zu verteidigen. Für Personen aus Westdeutschland war diese Reaktion nicht zu beobachten. Weiterhin zeigte sich, dass jüngere Personen aus Ostdeutschland die DM nach Mortalitätssalienz abwerteten, während bei älteren Personen eine tendenzielle Aufwertung zu beobachten war. Diese Befunde verdeutlichen, dass Reaktionen auf nationale Ereignisse und Symbole unter Mortalitätssalienz in Abhängigkeit unterschiedlicher Bevölkerungs- und Altersgruppen differenziert betrachtet werden müssen.


Abstract: Research on terror management theory suggests that mortality salience intensifies the desire to defend one's own cultural worldview against threats. In US-American studies in particular this has been reflected in increased defense of national symbols. We report a study that investigated the influence of mortality salience on reactions of people in East and West Germany toward defense of the German reunification and attitudes towards the German Mark (DM). Our results show that following mortality salience participants from East Germany exhibited an increased tendency to defend the German reunification against threat compared to a control condition. West-German participants did not show this reaction. In addition, the results show that following mortality salience younger East-German participants devalued the DM, whereas older participants tended to report increased liking of the DM. The current findings suggest that different sections of a population and different age groups have to be taken into account when analyzing reactions to national events and symbols following mortality salience.

References

  • Aiken, L.S. , West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions . Newbury Park, CA: Sage . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Arndt, J. , Greenberg, J. , Cook, A. (2002). Mortality salience and the spreading activation of worldview-relevant constructs: Exploring the cognitive architecture of terror management. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 307– 324 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Arndt, J. , Greenberg, J. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. (1997). Subliminal exposure to death-related stimuli increases defense of the cultural worldview. Psychological Science, 8, 379– 385 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Arndt, J. , Greenberg, J. , Solomon, S. , Pyszczynski, T. , Schimel, J. (1999). Creativity and terror management: Evidence that creative activity increases guilt and social projection following mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 19– 32 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Arndt, J. , Greenberg, J. , Solomon, S. , Pyszczynski, T. , Simon, L. (1997). Suppression, accessibility of death-related thoughts, and cultural worldview defense: Exploring the psychodynamics of terror management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 5– 18 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Buono, A.F. , Bowditch, J.L. , Lewis III, J.W. (1985). When cultures collide: The anatomy of a merger. Human Relations, 38, 477– 500 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Castano, E. , Yzerbyt, V. , Paladino, M.-O. , Sacchi, S. (2002). I belong, therefore, I exist: Ingroup identification, ingroup entitativity, and ingroup bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 135– 143 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dechesne, M. , Pyszczynski, T. , Arndt, J. , Ransom, S. , Sheldon, K.M. , van Knippenberg, A. , Janssen, J. (2003). Literal and symbolic immortality. The effect of evidence of literal immortality on self-esteem striving in response to mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 722– 737 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dehm, H. , Müller-Peters, A. (2001). The impact of economic representations and national identity on the attitude towards the euro. In C. Roland-Lévy, E. Kirchler, E. Penz & C. Gray (Eds.), Everyday representations of the economy (pp. 205-231). Wien: Universitätsverlag . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Arndt, J. , Schimel, J. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. (2001). Clarifying the function of mortality-salience induced worldview defense: Renewed suppression or reduced accessibility of death-related thoughts?. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 70– 76 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Arndt, J. , Simon, L. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. (2000). Proximal and distal defenses in response to reminders of one's mortality: Evidence of a temporal sequence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 91– 99 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Porteus, J. , Simon, L. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. (1995). Evidence of a terror management function of cultural icons: The effects of mortality salience on the inappropriate use of cherished cultural symbols. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1221– 1228 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. , Pinel, E. , Simon, L. , Jordan, K. (1993). Effects of self-esteem on vulnerability-denying defensive distortions: Further evidence of an anxiety-buffering function of self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 229– 251 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. , Rosenblatt, A. , Veeder, M. , Kirkland, S. , Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence for terror management theory II: The effects of mortality salience reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 308– 318 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. , Simon, L. , Breus, M. (1994). The role of consciousness and accessibility of death-related thoughts in mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 627– 637 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Simon, L. , Harmon-Jones, E. , Solomon, S. , Pyszczynski, T. , Lyon, D. (1995). Testing alternative explanations for mortality salience effects: Terror management, value accessibility, or worrisome thoughts?. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 417– 433 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Simon, L. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. , Chatel, D. (1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does mortality salience always intensify negative reactions to others who threaten one's worldview?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 212– 220 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenberg, J. , Solomon, S. , Pyszczynski, T (1997). Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. In M.P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 61-139). San Diego: Academic Press . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Harmon-Jones, E. , Simon, L. , Greenberg, J. , Pyszczynski, T. , Solomon, S. , McGregor, H. (1997). Terror management theory and self-esteem: Evidence that increased self-esteem reduces mortality salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 24– 36 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Helleiner, E. (1998). National currencies and national identities. American Behavioral Scientist, 41, 1409– 1436 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jonas, E. (2004). Unpublished data, University of Munich, Germany . . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jonas, E. , Fritsche, I. , Greenberg, J. (2005). Currencies as cultural symbols - an existential psychological perspective on reactions of Germans toward the Euro. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26, 129– 146 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jonas, E. , Greenberg, J. (2004). Terror management and political attitudes: The influence of mortality salience on Germans' defence of the German reunification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 1– 9 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jonas, E. , Schimel, J. , Greenberg, J. , Pyszczynski, T. (2002). The Scrooge effect: Evidence that mortality salience increases prosocial attitudes and behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1342– 1353 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kessler, T. , Hollbach, S. (in press). Group-based emotions as determinants of ingroup identification. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Müller-Peters, A. (1998). The significance of national pride and national identity to the attitude toward the single european currency: A Europe-wide comparison. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19, 701– 719 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nelson, L.J. , Moore, D.L. , Olivetti, J. , Scott, T. (1997). General and personal mortality salience and nationalistic bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 884– 892 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ochsmann, R. , Mathy, M. (1994). Depreciating of and distancing from foreigners: Effects of mortality salience . Unpublished manuscript, Universität Mainz, Mainz, Germany . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmitt, M. , Maes, J. (2002). Stereotypic ingroup bias as self-defense against relative deprivation: Evidence from a longitudinal study of the German unification process. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 309– 326 . First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Solomon, S. , Greenberg, J. , Pyszczynski, T. (2004). The cultural animal: Twenty years of terror management theory and research. In J. Greenberg, S. Koole & T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential psychology (pp. 13-34). New York: Guilford . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ramelsberger, A. (2003). Kein schöner Land vor ihrer Zeit . [online]. Verfügbar unter: www.goethe.de/kug/ges/ztg/thm/de54689.htm [26.01.2004] . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rau, J. (2002). Rede von Bundespräsident Johannes Rau beim Festakt zum Jahrestag der Deutschen Einheit im Konzerthaus am Gendarmenmarkt Berlin am 03.10.2002 . [online]. Verfügbar unter: www.bundespraesident.de/frameset/index.jsp [23.02.2004] . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rheingold Institut (2001). Bis ins Mark erschüttert: Die psychologischen Folgen der Währungsumstellung. [online]. Innovations Report. Forum für Wissenschaft, Industrie und Wirtschaft. Verfügbar unter: www.innovations-report.de/html/berichte/wirtschaft_finanzen/bericht-3052.html [26.01.2004] . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D. , Clark, L.A. (1994). The PANAS-X. Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule - expanded form . The University of Iowa (updated 8/99) . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wilberg, S. (1996). Favorisierung der “eigenen” Gruppe in einem deutsch-deutschen Vergleich. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 27, 278– 282 . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar