Skip to main content
Short Research Report

Talk or Walk? Gait Speed over Self-Report in Association with Cognitive Speed in Healthy Older Adults

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1662-9647/a000202

Abstract. This study explores the relative value of both subjectively reported cognitive speed and gait speed in association with objectively derived cognitive speed. It also explores how these factors are affected by psychological and physical well-being. A group of 90 cognitively healthy older adults (M = 73.38, SD = 8.06 years, range = 60–89 years) were tested in a three-task cognitive battery to determine objective cognitive speed as well as measures of gait speed, well-being, and subjective cognitive speed. Analyses indicated that gait speed was associated with objective cognitive speed to a greater degree than was subjective report, the latter being more closely related to well-being than to objective cognitive speed. These results were largely invariant across the 30-year age range of our older adult sample.

References

  • Abellan van Kan, G., Rolland, Y., Andrieu, S., Bauer, J., Beauchet, M., Bonnefoy, M. C., … Visser, B. V. (2009). Gait speed at usual pace as a predictor of adverse outcomes in community-dwelling older people an International Academy on Nutrition and Aging (IANA) Task Force. The Journal of Nutrition, Health, and Aging, 13, 881–889. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Atkinson, H. H., Rosano, C., Simonsick, E. M., Williamson, J. D., Davis, C., Ambrosius, W. T., … Kritchevsky, S. B. (2007). Cognitive function, gait speed decline, and comorbidities: The health, aging and body composition study. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 62, 844–850. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bach, M. (1996). The Freiburg Visual Acuity Test–Automatic measurement of visual acuity. Optometry and Vision Science, 73, 49–53. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Balash, Y., Mordechovich, M., Shabtai, H., Giladi, N., Gurevich, T. & Korczyn, A.D. (2013). Subjective memory complaints in elders: Depression, anxiety, or cognitive decline? Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 127, 344–350. https://doi.org/10.1111/ane.12038 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baltes, P.B. & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of aging: From successful aging of the young old to the dilemmas of the fourth age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bartzokis, G., Lu, P. H., Tingus, K., Mendez, M. F., Richard, A., Peters, D. G., … Mintz, J. (2010). Lifespan trajectory of myelin integrity and maximum motor speed. Neurobiology of Aging, 31, 1554–1562. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bashore, T.R., Ridderinkhof, K.R. & van der Molen, M.W. (1997). The decline of cognitive processing speed in old age. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 163–169. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bherer, L., Erickson, K.I. & Lui-Ambrose, T. (2013). A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. Journal of Aging Research, 1–8. ID 657508 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Buelow, M.T., Tremont, G., Frakey, L.L., Grace, J. & Ott, B.R. (2014). Utility of the cognitive difficulties scale and association with objective test performance. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 29, 755–761. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burmester, B., Leathem, J. & Merrick, P. (2016). Subjective cognitive complaints and objective cognitive function in aging: A systematic review and meta-analysis of recent cross-sectional findings. Neuropsychological Review, 26, 376–393. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Burmester, B., Leathem, J. & Merrick, P. (2017). Influence of assessment methods on subjective and objective memory impairment. GeroPsych, 30, 79–84. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Calabria, M., Manenti, R., Rosini, S., Zanetti, O., Miniussi, C. & Cotelli, M. (2011). Objective and subjective memory impairment in elderly adults: A revised version of the everyday memory questionnaire. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 23, 67–73. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Callisaya, M.L., Blizzard, C.L., Wood, A.G., Thrift, A.G., Wardill, T. & Srikanth, V.K. (2015). Longitudinal relationships between cognitive decline and gait slowing: The Tasmanian study of cognition and gait. Journals of Gerontology A: Biological Sciences Medical Sciences, 70, 1226–1232. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glv066 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Caramelli, P. & Beato, R. G. (2008). Subjective memory complaints and memory performance in a sample of healthy elderly. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 2(1), 42–45. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chin, J., Oh, K. J., Seo, S. W. & Na, D. L. (2014). Are depressive symptomatology and self-focused attention associated with subjective memory impairment in older adults? International Psychogeriatrics, 26, 573–580. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cho, H. C. & Abe, S. (2013). Is two-tailed testing for directional research hypotheses tests legitimate? Journal of Business Research, 66, 1261–1266. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clément, F., Belleville, S. & Gauthier, S. (2008). Cognitive complaint in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 222–232. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G. & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York, NY: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cook, S. & Marsiske, M. (2006). Subjective memory beliefs and cognitive performance in normal and mildly impaired older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 10, 413–423. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cosetino, S., Devandand, D. & Gurland, B. (2018). A link between subjective perceptions of memory and physical function: Implications for subjective cognitive decline. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 61, 1387–1398. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170495 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Costello, M. C. & Bloesch, E. K. (2017). Are older adults less embodied? A review of age effects through the lens of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 267. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00267 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crumley, J. J., Stelter, C. A. & Horhota, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between subjective and objective memory performance in older adults: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 29, 250–263. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035908 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cutler, S. J. & Grams, A. E. (1988). Correlates of self-reported everyday memory problems. Journal of Gerontology, 43, S82–S90. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/43.3.S82 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dolan, R. J. (2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science, 298, 1191–1194. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duits, A., Munnecom, T., van Heugten, C. & van Oostenbrugge, R. J. (2008). Cognitive complaints in the early phase after stroke are not indicative of cognitive impairment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 79, 143–146. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eckert, M.A. (2011). Slowing down: Age-related neurobiological predictors of processing speed. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 24. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2011.00025 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Edwards, J. R. (2001). Ten difference score myths. Organizational Research Methods, 4, 265–287. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Feise, R. J. (2002). Do multiple outcome measures require p-value adjustment? BMC Medical Research Methodology, 2, 8. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Folstein, M., Folstein, S. & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • George, B. J., Beasley, T. M., Brown, A. W., Dawson, J., Dimova, R., Divers, J., … Kim, M. Y. (2016). Common scientific and statistical errors in obesity research. Obesity, 24, 781–790. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hillman, C.H., Erickson, K. I. & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 58–65. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hofer, M. & Allemand, M. (2017). Dispositional and situational emotion regulation in younger and older adults. GeroPsych: The Journal of Gerontopsychology and Geriatric Psychiatry, 30, 109–118. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hohman, T. J., Beason-Held, L. L., Lamar, M. & Resnick, S. M. (2011). Subjective cognitive complaints and longitudinal changes in memory and brain function. Neuropsychology, 25, 125–130. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holtzer, R., Mahoney, J. & Verghese, J. (2014). Intraindividual variability in executive functions but not speed of processing or conflict resolution predicts performance differences in gait speed in older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences Medical Sciences, 69, 980–986. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hülür, G., Willis, S.L., Hertzog, C., Schaie, K.W. & Gerstorf, D. (2018). Is subjective memory specific for memory performance or general across cognitive domains? Findings from the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Psychology and Aging, 33, 448–460. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jopp, D. & Hertzog, C. (2007). Activities, self-referent memory beliefs, and cognitive performance: Evidence for direct and mediated relations. Psychology and Aging, 22, 811–825. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jungwirth, S., Fischer, P., Weissgram, S., Kirchmeyr, W., Bauer, P. & Tragl, K-H. (2004). Subjective memory complaints and objective memory impairment in the Vienna-Transdanube aging community. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52, 263–268. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52066.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kensinger, E. & Gutchess, A. H. (2017). Cognitive aging in a social and affective context: advances over the past 50 years. The Journal of Gerontology: Series B, 72(1), 61–70. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kerchner, G. A., Racine, C. A., Hale, S., Wilheim, R., Laluz, V., Miller, B. L. & Kramer, J.H. (2012). Cognitive processing speed in older adults: Relationship with white matter integrity. PLoS ONE, 7(11), e50425. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0050425 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kimmel, H. D. (1957). Three criteria for the use of one-tailed tests. Psychological Bulletin, 54, 351–353. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kraft, E. (2012). Cognitive function, physical activity, and aging: Possible biological links and implications for multimodal interventions. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19, 248–263. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laming, D. (1979). A critical comparison of two random-walk models for two-choice reaction time. Acta Psychologica, 43, 431–453. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lawton, M. P. & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist, 9, 179–186. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mabe, P. A. & West, S. G. (1982). Validity of self-evaluation of ability: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 280–296. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.67.3.280 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacDonald, S. W. S., Hundza, A., Love, J.A., DeCarlo, C.A., Halliday, D. W. R., Brewster, P. W. H., … Dixon, R.A. (2017). Concurrent indicators of gait velocity and variability are associated with 25-year cognitive change: A Retrospective longitudinal investigation. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2017.00017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, K.L., Blizzard, L., Wood, A.G., Srikanth, V., Thomson, R., Sanders, L.M. & Callisaya, M.L. (2013). Cognitive function, gait, and gait variability in older people: A population-based study. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 68, 726–732. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/gls224 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McClelland, G. H. & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 376–390. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Minett, T. S. C., Da Silva, R. V., Ortiz, K. Z. & Bertolucci, P. H. F. (2007). Subjective memory complaints in an elderly sample: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23, 49–54. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1836 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nyström, M. B. T., Sörman, D. E., Kormi-Nouri, R. & Rönnlund, M. (2017). To what extent is subjective well-being in late adulthood related to subjective and objective memory functioning? Five-year cross-lagged panel analyses. Aging & Mental Health, . https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2017.1394439 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • O’Keefe, D. J. (2003). Colloquy: Should familywise alpha be adjusted? Against familywise alpha adjustment. Human Communication Research, 29, 431–447. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Payne, B. R., Gross, A. L., Hill, P. L., Parisi, J. M., Rebok, G. W. & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2016). Decomposing the relationship between cognitive functioning and self-referent memory beliefs in older adulthood: What’s memory got to do with it? Aging, Neuropsychologyand Cognition, 24, 345–362. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Quan, M., Xun, P., Chen, C., Wen, J., Wang, Y., Wang, R., … He, K. (2017). Walking pace and the risk of cognitive decline and dementia in elderly populations: A meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 72, 266–270. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reid, L. M. & MacLullich, A. M. J. (2006). Subjective memory complaints and cognitive impairment in older people. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 22, 471–485. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riedel-Heller, S. G., Matschinger, H., Schork, A. & Angermeyer, M.C. (1999). Do memory complaints indicate the presence of cognitive impairment? Results of a field study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 249, 197–204. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roberg, B. L., Bruce, J. M., Lovelace, C. T. & Lynch, S. (2012). How patients with multiple sclerosis perceive cognitive slowing. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26, 1278–1295. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salthouse, T. A. (1992). What do adult age differences in the Digit Symbol Substitution Test reflect? Journal of Gerontology, 47, 121–128. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/47.3.P121 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salthouse, T. A. (2010). Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16, 754–760. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Santo, H. E. & Daniel, F. (2018). Optimism and well-being among institutionalized older adults. GeroPsych, 31, 5–16. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M. & Lindenberger, U. (2014). Younger adults show long-term effects of cognitive training on broad cognitive abilities over 2 years. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2304–2310. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Seidler, A. L. & Wolff, J. K. (2017). Bidirectional associations between self-perceptions of aging and processing speed across 3 years. GeroPsych, 30, 49–59. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, J. & Baltes, P.B. (1997). Profiles of psychological functioning in the old and oldest old. Psychology and Aging, 12, 458–472. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Snitz, B. E., Small, B. J., Wang, T., Change, C-C. H., Hughes, T. F. & Ganguli, M. (2015). Do subjective memory complaints lead or follow objective cognitive change? A five-year population study of temporal influence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21, 732–742. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sofi, F., Valecchi, D., Bacci, D., Abbate, R., Gensisi, G.F., Casini, A. & Macchi, C. (2010). Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Journal of Internal Medicine, 269, 107–117. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Soumare, A., Tavernier, B., Alperovitch, A., Tzourio, C. & Elbaz, A. (2009). A cross-sectional and longitudinal study of the relationship between walking speed and cognitive function in community-dwelling elderly people. Journal of Internal Medicine, 10, 1058–1065. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glp077 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Spector, P. E. & Jex, S. M. (1998). Development of four self-report measures of job stressors and strain: interpersonal conflict at work scale, organizational constraints scale, quantitative workload inventory, and physical symptoms inventory. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 3, 356–367. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tabbarah, M., Crimmins, E. M. & Seeman, T. E. (2002). The relationship between cognitive and physical performance: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 57, 228–235. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/57.4.M228 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tobiansky, R., Blizard, R., Livingston, G. & Mann, A. (1995). The Gospel Oak Study stage IV: The clinical relevance of subjective impairment in older people. Psychological Medicine, 25, 779–786. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tukey, J.W. (1977). Some thoughts on clinical trials, especially problems of multiplicity. Science, 198, 679–684. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tweedy, J. R., Lapinski, R. H. & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1977). Semantic-context effects on word recognition: Influence of varying the proportion of items in an appropriate context. Memory & Cognition, 5, 84–89. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, N. L., Rosano, C., Boudreau, R. M., Simonsick, E. M., Ferrucci, L., Sutton-Tyrrell, K., … Newman, A. B. (2010). Executive function, memory, and gait speed decline in well-functioning older adults. The Journals of Gerontology, 65a(10), 1093–1100. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/g1q111 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Weaver, B. & Wuensch, K. L. (2013). SPSS and SAS programs for comparing Pearson correlations and OLS regression coefficients. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 880–895. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Revised. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Young, J., Anstey, K. J. & Cherbuin, N. (2012). Online memory screening: Are older adults interested and can it work? Aging and Mental Health, 16, 931–937. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2012.684667 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zou, G. (2007). Toward using confidence intervals to compare correlations. Psychological Methods, 12, 399–413. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.12.4.399 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar