Skip to main content
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000183

Cognitive psychology offers an important contribution to the understanding of the mechanisms underlying intelligence. In this paper, we synthesize the research showing that, among the different cognitive mechanisms associated with intelligence, working memory has a particularly high explanatory power, especially when considered in its active component involving not only the maintenance (as in short-term memory) but also the manipulation of information. The paper considers two main implications of this finding for the applied and clinical fields. For a start, we examine how intelligence tests take into consideration working memory. Secondly, we consider the highly debated literature on the effects of working memory training on intellectual performance. Theoretical and applied implications for the relationship between working memory and intelligence are discussed.

References

  • Ackerman, P. L. , Beier, M. E. , Boyle, M. O. (2005). Working memory and intelligence: The same or different constructs? Psychological Bulletin, 131, 30–60. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.30. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alloway, T. P. , Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 20–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.11.003. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alloway, T. P. , Gathercole, S. E. , Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development, 77, 1698–1716. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00968.x. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alloway, T. P. , Passolunghi, M. C. (2011). The relationship between working memory, IQ, and mathematical skills in children. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 133–137. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.09.013. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. D. , Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. A. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 47–89). New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barnett, S. M. , Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn? A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 612–637. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.612. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beck, S. J. , Hanson, C. A. , Puffenberger, S. S. , Benninger, K. L. , Benninger, W. B. (2010). A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 825–836. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517162. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Belacchi, C. , Carretti, B. , Cornoldi, C. (2010). The role of working memory and updating in Coloured Raven Matrices performance in typically developing children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1010–1020. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bittner, A. C. , Carter, R. C. , Kennedy, R. S. , Harbeson, M. M. , Krause, M. (1986). Performance evaluation tests for environmental research (peter): Evaluation of 114 measures. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 63, 683–708. doi: 10.2466/pms.1986.63.2.683. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Borella, E. , Carretti, B. , Riboldi, F. , De Beni, R. (2010). Working memory training in older adults: Evidence of transfer and maintenance effects. Psychology and Aging, 25, 767–778. doi: 10.1037/a0020683. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brydges, C. R. , Reid, C. L. , Fox, A. M. , Anderson, M. (2012). A unitary executive function predicts intelligence in children. Intelligence, 40, 458–469. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.05.006. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carretti, B. , Belacchi, C. , Cornoldi, C. (2010). Difficulties in working memory updating in individuals with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 337–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01267. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carretti, B. , Borella, E. , Zavagnin, M. , de Beni, R. (2013). Gains in language comprehension relating to working memory training in healthy older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28, 539–546. doi: 10.1002/gps.3859. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: A systematic reinterpretation. New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chan, R. C. K. , Shum, D. , Toulopoulou, T. , Chen, E. Y. H. (2008). Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chooi, W.-T. , Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence, 40, 531–542. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.004. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clarke, P. J. , Snowling, M. J. , Truelove, E. , Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s reading-comprehension difficulties: A randomized controlled trial. Psychological Science, 21, 1106–1116. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Colom, R. , Rebollo, I. , Abad, F. J. , Shih, P. C. (2006). Complex span tasks, simple span tasks, and cognitive abilities: A reanalysis of key studies. Memory and Cognition, 34, 158–171. doi: 10.3758/BF03193395. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Conway, A. R. A. , Getz, S. J. , Macnamara, B. , Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. (2011). Working memory and fluid intelligence: A multi-mechanism view. In Sternberg, R. J. , Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of intelligence. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cornoldi, C. (2006). The contribution of cognitive psychology to the study of human intelligence. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 1–17. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cornoldi, C. (2007). L’intelligenza [Intelligence]. Bologna, Italy: Il Mulino. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cornoldi, C. (2010). Metacognition, intelligence, and academic performance. In Salatas Waters, H. , Schneider, W. (Eds.), Metacognition, strategy use, and instruction (pp. 257–277). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cornoldi, C. , Giofrè, D. , Calgaro, G. , Stupiggia, C. (2013). Attentional WM is not necessarily specifically related with fluid intelligence: The case of smart children with ADHD symptoms. Psychological Research, 77, 508–515. doi: 10.1007/s00426-012-0446-8. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cornoldi, C. , Giofrè, D. , Martini, A. (2013). Problems in deriving Italian regional differences in intelligence from 2009 PISA data. Intelligence, 41, 25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.10.004. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cornoldi, C. , Orsini, A. , Cianci, L. , Giofrè, D. , Pezzuti, L. (2013). Intelligence and working memory control: Evidence from the WISC-IV administration to Italian children. Learning and Individual Differences, 26, 9–14. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.04.005. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cornoldi, C. , Vecchi, T. (2003). Visuo-spatial working memory and individual differences. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Coyle, T. R. (2003). IQ, the worst performance rule, and Spearman’s law: A reanalysis and extension. Intelligence, 31, 473–489. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00175-7. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Deary, I. J. , Penke, L. , Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 11, 201–211. doi: 10.1038/nrn2793. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, J. (2005). Frontal lobe function and general intelligence: Why it matters. Cortex, 41, 215–217. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70896-7. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duncan, J. , Burgess, P. , Emslie, H. (1995). Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 33, 261–268. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00124-8. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 19–23. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00160. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Engle, R. W. (2010). Role of working memory capacity in cognitive control. Current Anthropology, 51, S17–S26. doi: 10.1086/650572. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Engle, R. W. , Tuholski, S. W. , Laughlin, J. E. , Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–331. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.3.309. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Evers, A. , Muñiz, J. , Bartram, D. , Boben, D. , Egeland, J. , Fernández-Hermida, J. R. , Urbànek, T. (2012). Testing practices in the 21st century. European Psychologist, 17, 300–319. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000102. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Fitzpatrick, C. , Pagani, L. S. (2012). Toddler working memory skills predict kindergarten school readiness. Intelligence, 40, 205–212. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2011.11.007. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, N. P. , Miyake, A. , Corley, R. P. , Young, S. E. , Defries, J. C. , Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172–179. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Galton, F. (1869). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London, UK: Macmillan/Fontana. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • García-Madruga, J. A. , Elosúa, M. R. , Gil, L. , Gómez-Veiga, I. , Vila, J. Ó. , Orjales, I. , Duque, G. (2013). Reading comprehension and working memory’s executive processes: An intervention study in primary school students. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 155–174. doi: 10.1002/rrq.44. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gathercole, S. E. , Dunning, D. L. , Holmes, J. (2012). Cogmed training: Let’s be realistic about intervention research. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 201–203. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.07.007. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Giofrè, D. , Mammarella, I. C. , Cornoldi, C. (2013). The structure of working memory and how it relates to intelligence in children. Intelligence, 41, 396–406. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.006. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gottfredson, L. (1997). Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence, 24, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grudnik, J. L. , Kranzler, J. H. (2001). Meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and inspection time. Intelligence, 29, 523–535. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00078-2. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hartman, E. , Houwen, S. , Scherder, E. , Visscher, C. (2010). On the relationship between motor performance and executive functioning in children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 54, 468–477. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01284.x. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Holmes, J. , Gathercole, S. E. , Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12, F9–F15. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00848.x. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hongwanishkul, D. , Happaney, K. R. , Lee, W. S. C. , Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: Age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28, 617–644. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_4. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hunt, E. (1980). Intelligence as an information-processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 449–474. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb01760.x. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hunt, E. (2011). Human intelligence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jaeggi, S. M. , Buschkuehl, M. , Jonides, J. , Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 6829–6833. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0801268105. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jaeggi, S. M. , Buschkuehl, M. , Jonides, J. , Shah, P. (2011). Short- and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 10081–10086. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1103228108. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jaeggi, S. M. , Studer-Luethi, B. , Buschkuehl, M. , Su, Y.-F. , Jonides, J. , Perrig, W. J. (2010). The relationship between n-back performance and matrix reasoning – implications for training and transfer. Intelligence, 38, 625–635. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2010.09.001. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jaušovec, N. , Jaušovec, K. (2012). Working memory training: Improving intelligence – changing brain activity. Brain and Cognition, 79, 96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.007. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g-factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CN: Praeger. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, A. R. (2006). Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences (1st ed.). Oxford, UK: Elsevier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Just, M. A. , Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122–149. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kane, M. J. , Engle, R. W. (2000). Working-memory capacity, proactive interference, and divided attention: Limits on long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 336–358. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.2.336. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kane, M. J. , Hambrick, D. Z. , Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence are strongly related constructs: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 66–71. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.66. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kane, M. J. , Hambrick, D. Z. , Tuholski, S. W. , Wilhelm, O. , Payne, T. W. , Engle, R. W. (2004). The generality of working memory capacity: A latent-variable approach to verbal and visuospatial memory span and reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 189–217. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 317–324. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.002. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klingberg, T. , Fernell, E. , Olesen, P. J. , Johnson, M. , Gustafsson, P. , Dahlström, K. , Wetserberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD – a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 177–186. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klingberg, T. , Forssberg, H. , Westerberg, H. (2002). Training of working memory in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 781–791. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.6.781.8395. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kochanska, G. , Murray, K. , Jacques, T. Y. , Koenig, A. L. , Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization. Child Development, 67, 490. doi: 10.2307/1131828. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kyllonen, P. C. , Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?!. Intelligence, 14, 389–433. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lanfranchi, S. , Cornoldi, C. , Drigo, S. , Vianello, R. (2009). Working memory in individuals with fragile X syndrome. Child Neuropsychology, 15, 105–119. doi: 10.1080/09297040802112564. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lanfranchi, S. , Cornoldi, C. , Vianello, R. (2004). Verbal and visuospatial working memory deficits in children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 109, 456–466. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2004)109<456. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martínez, K. , Colom, R. (2009). Working memory capacity and processing efficiency predict fluid but not crystallized and spatial intelligence: Evidence supporting the neural noise hypothesis. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 281–286. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.012. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meinz, E. J. , Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). Deliberate practice is necessary but not sufficient to explain individual differences in piano sight-reading skill: The role of working memory capacity. Psychological Science, 21, 914–919. doi: 10.1177/0956797610373933. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Meinz, E. J. , Hambrick, D. Z. , Hawkins, C. B. , Gillings, A. K. , Meyer, B. E. , Schneider, J. L. (2011). Roles of domain knowledge and working memory capacity in components of skill in Texas Hold’Em poker. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2011.11.001. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Melby-Lervåg, M. , Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49, 270–291. doi: 10.1037/a0028228. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mezzacappa, E. , Buckner, J. C. (2010). Working memory training for children with attention problems or hyperactivity: A school-based pilot study. School Mental Health, 2, 202–208. doi: 10.1007/s12310-010-9030-9. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A. , Friedman, N. P. , Emerson, M. J. , Witzki, A. H. , Howerter, A. , Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp. 1999.0734. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nisbett, R. E. , Aronson, J. , Blair, C. , Dickens, W. , Flynn, J. , Halpern, D. F. , Turkheimer, E. (2012). Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. American Psychologist, 67, 130–159. doi: 10.1037/a0026699. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oberauer, K. , Schulze, R. , Wilhelm, O. , Süss, H.-M. (2005). Working memory and intelligence – their correlation and their relation: Comment on Ackerman, Beier, and Boyle (2005). Psychological Bulletin, 131, 61–65. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.61. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Owen, A. M. , Hampshire, A. , Grahn, J. A. , Stenton, R. , Dajani, S. , Burns, A. S. , Ballard, C. G. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature, 465, 775–778. doi: 10.1038/nature09042. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Palladino, P. , Cornoldi, C. (2004). Working memory performance of Italian students with foreign language learning difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 14, 137–151. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Prifitera, A. , Weiss, L. G. , Saklofske, D. (1998). The WISC-III in context. In Prifitera, A. , Saklofske, D. H. (Eds.), WISC-III clinical use and interpretation: Scientist-practitioner perspectives. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Raven, J. , Raven, J. C. , Court, J. H. (1998). Raven manual, Section 2 (Coloured Progressive Matrices). Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologist Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Reams, R. , Chamrad, D. , Robinson, N. M. (1990). The race is not necessarily to the swift: Validity of WISC-R bonus points for speed. Gifted Child Quarterly, 34, 108–110. doi: 10.1177/001698629003400304. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Redick, T. S. , Shipstead, Z. , Harrison, T. L. , Hicks, K. L. , Fried, D. E. , Hambrick, D. Z. , Engle, R. W. (2013). No evidence of intelligence improvement after working memory training: A randomized, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 359–379. doi: 10.1037/a0029082. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Redick, T. S. , Unsworth, N. , Kelly, A. J. , Engle, R. W. (2012). Faster, smarter? Working memory capacity and perceptual speed in relation to fluid intelligence. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 844–854. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2012.704359. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roca, M. , Parr, A. , Thompson, R. , Woolgar, A. , Torralva, T. , Antoun, N. , Duncan, J. (2010). Executive function and fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Brain, 133, 234–247. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp269. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, fifth edition examiner’s manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ruthsatz, J. , Urbach, J. B. (2012). Child prodigy: A novel cognitive profile places elevated general intelligence, exceptional working memory and attention to detail at the root of prodigiousness. Intelligence, 40, 419–426. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2012.06.002. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shipstead, Z. , Hicks, K. L. , Engle, R. W. (2012). Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence support the claims? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1, 185–193. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.06.003. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shipstead, Z. , Redick, T. S. , Engle, R. W. (2010). Does working memory training generalize? Psychologica Belgica, 50, 245–276. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shipstead, Z. , Redick, T. S. , Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138, 628–654. doi: 10.1037/a0027473. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Söderqvist, S. , Bergman Nutley, S. , Peyrard-Janvid, M. , Matsson, H. , Humphreys, K. , Kere, J. , Klingberg, T. (2012). Dopamine, working memory, and training induced plasticity: Implications for developmental research. Developmental Psychology, 48, 836–843. doi: 10.1037/a0026179. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stevenson, C. E. , Hickendorff, M. , Resing, W. C. M. , Heiser, W. J. , De Boeck, P. A. L. (2013). Explanatory item response modeling of children’s change on a dynamic test of analogical reasoning. Intelligence, 41, 157–168. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.01.003. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Süß, H. M. , Oberauer, K. , Wittmann, W. W. , Wilhelm, O. , Schulze, R. (2002). Working-memory capacity explains reasoning ability – and a little bit more. Intelligence, 30, 261–288. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(01)00100-3. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swanson, H. L. , Siegel, L. (2001). Learning disabilities as a working memory deficit. Issues in Education, 7, 1–48. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Todd, J. J. , Marois, R. (2004). Capacity limit of visual short-term memory in human posterior parietal cortex. Nature, 428, 751–754. doi: 10.1038/nature02466. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unsworth, N. , Brewer, G. A. , Spillers, G. J. (2009). There’s more to the working memory capacity-fluid intelligence relationship than just secondary memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16, 931–937. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.5.931. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unsworth, N. , Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory capacity and fluid abilities: Examining the correlation between Operation Span and Raven. Intelligence, 33, 67–81. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.08.003. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unsworth, N. , Engle, R. W. (2006). A temporal-contextual retrieval account of complex span: An analysis of errors. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 346–362. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.11.004. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Unsworth, N. , Engle, R. W. (2007). On the division of short-term and working memory: An examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1038–1066. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.6.1038. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wechsler, D. (2004). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th ed.). London, UK: Pearson Assessment. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition: Technical and interpretive manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Weiss, L. G. , Saklofske, D. H. , Prifitera, A. , Holdnack, J. A. (2006). WISC-IV. Advanced clinical interpretation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilhelm, O. , Engle, R. W. (2005). Handbook of understanding and measuring intelligence. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilhelm, O. , Oberauer, K. (2006). Why are reasoning ability and working memory capacity related to mental speed? An investigation of stimulus-response compatibility in choice reaction time tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 18–50. doi: 10.1080/09541440500215921. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wilhelm, O. , Schulze, R. (2002). The relation of speeded and unspeeded reasoning with mental speed. Intelligence, 30, 537–554. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00086-7. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar