Skip to main content
Original Articles and Reviews

Delay Discounting in Impulsive Behavior

A Systematic Review

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000360

Abstract. Delay discounting (DD) is the process of devaluing results that happen in the future. With this review, we intend to identify specificities in the processes of DD in impulsive behavior. Studies were retrieved from multiple literature databases, through rigorous criteria (we included systematic reviews and empirical studies with adult human subjects), following the procedures of the Cochrane Collaboration initiative. Of the 174 documents obtained, 19 were considered eligible for inclusion and were retained for in-depth analysis. In addition, 13 studies from the manual search were included. Thus, a total of 32 studies were selected for review. The objectives/hypotheses, results, and the main conclusion(s) were extracted from each study. Results show that people with pronounced traits of impulsivity discount rewards more markedly, that is, they prefer immediate rewards, though of less value, or postponed losses, even though they worsen in the future. Taken together, the existing data suggest the importance of inserting DD as a tool for initial assessment in conjunction with measures of addiction and stress level, as well as the consideration of new therapies.

References

  • Ainslie, G. (1974). Impulse control in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 485–489. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alcorn, J. III, Gowin, J., Green, C., Swann, A., Moeller, F., & Lane, S. (2013). Aggression, impulsivity, and psychopathic traits in combined antisocial personality disorder and substance use disorder. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 25, 229–232. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.12030060 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alessi, S., & Petry, N. (2003). Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behavioural Processes, 64, 345–354. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00150-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Amlung, M., & MacKillop, J. (2014). Clarifying the relationship between impulsive delay discounting and nicotine dependence. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28, 761–768. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036726 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bickel, W., & Johnson, M. (2003). Delay discounting: A fundamental behavioral process of drug dependence. In G. LoewensteinD. ReadR. BaumeisterEds., Time and decision: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice (pp. 419–440). New York, NY: APA. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bickel, W., Odum, A., & Madden, G. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 447–454. https://doi.org/10.1007/PL00005490 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bickel, W., Yi, R., Landes, R., Hill, P., & Baxter, C. (2011). Remember the future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 260–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Black, A., & Rosen, M. (2011). A money management-based substance use treatment increases valuation of future rewards. Addictive Behaviors, 36, 125–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.08.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brody, G., Yu, T., MacKillop, J., Miller, G., Chen, E., Obasi, E., … Beach, S. (2014). Catecholamine levels and delay discounting forecast drug use among african american youths. Addiction, 109, 1112–1118. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12516 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carroll, M., Anker, J., Mach, J., Newman, J., & Perry, J. (2010). Delay discounting as a predictor of drug abuse. In G. J. MaddenW. K. BickelEds., Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting (pp. 243–271). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/12069-009 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • da Matta, A., Gonçalves, F., & Bizarro, L. (2014). Desvalorização pelo atraso, dependência química e impulsividade [Delay discounting, substance addiction and impulsivity]. Avances en Psicología Latinoamericana, 32, 217–230. https://doi.org/10.12804/ap132.2.22014.03 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Damnjanovic, S. (2011). Dissociating facets of impulsivity: Executive control vs. intertemporal choice. Dissertation, University of Florida. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Daugherty, J., & Brase, G. (2010). Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 202–207. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Groot, F., & Dom, G. (2005). Impulsiviteit (delay discounting) en zelfcontrole: Een gedragsanalytische verkenning [Impulsivity (delay discounting) and self-control: A behavioral analysis]. Gedragstherapie, 38, 107–130. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • de Wit, H., & Mitchell, S. (2010). Drug effects on delay discounting. In G. J. MaddenW. K. BickelEds., Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting (pp. 213–241). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/12069-008 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Dixon, M., & Holcomb, S. (2000). Teaching self-control to small groups of dually diagnosed adults. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 611–614. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-611 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dixon, M., & Holton, B. (2009). Altering the magnitude of delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 269–275. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2009.42-269 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dixon, M., Jacobs, E., & Sanders, S. (2006). Contextual control of delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 413–422. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2006.173-05 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dixon, M., Marley, J., & Jacobs, E. (2003). Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 449–458. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2003.36-449 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gu, L. (2013). Delay discounting as a measure of impulsivity. Dissertation, University of Minnesota First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Heil, S., Johnson, M., Higgins, S., & Bickel, W. (2006). Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. Addictive Behaviors, 31, 1290–1294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.09.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Higgins, J., & Green, S. (2011). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. (Version 5.1.0). Retrieved from https://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hoffman, W., Moore, M., Templin, R., McFarland, B., Hitzemann, R., & Mitchell, S. (2006). Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology, 188, 162–170. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0494-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hollander, E., & Evers, M. (2001). New developments in impulsivity. Lancet, 358, 949–950. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(01)06114-1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, S., & Lee, D. (2011). Prefrontal cortex and impulsive decision making. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 1140–1146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kirby, K., Winston, G., & Santiesteban, M. (2005). Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 213–222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2005.01.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Landes, R., Christensen, D., & Bickel, W. (2012). Delay discounting decreases in those completing treatment for opioid dependence. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20, 302–309. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027391 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Landis, J., & Koch, G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529310 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • LaVigne, J. (2011). Assessment of delay discounting as a measure of impulsivity in problem video gamers. Michigan: UMI Dissertation Publishing. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Löckenhoff, C., O’Donoghue, T., & Dunning, D. (2011). Age differences in temporal discounting – the role of dispositional affect and anticipated emotions. Psychology and Aging, 26, 274–284. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023280 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Logue, A. (1988). Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 11, 665–709. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00053978 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • MacKillop, J., & Kahler, C. (2009). Delayed reward discounting predicts treatment response for heavy drinkers receiving smoking cessation treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 104, 197–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.04.020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Madden, G.Bickel, W. (2010). Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mobini, S., Grant, A., Kass, A., & Yeomans, M. (2007). Relationships between functional and dysfunctional impulsivity, delay discounting and cognitive distortions. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1517–1528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moreira, D., Barros, S., Almeida, F., Pinto, M., & Barbosa, F. (2016). Changes on intertemporal choices in deviant behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 344–347. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.046 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Odum, A. (2011a). Delay discounting: Trait variable. Behavioural Processes, 87, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.02.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Odum, A. (2011b). Delay discounting: I’m a k, You’re a k. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96, 427–439. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.96-423 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Öncüler, A., & Onay, S. (2009). How do we evaluate future gambles? Experimental evidence on path dependency in risky intertemporal choice. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 22, 280–300. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.626 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ortner, C., MacDonald, T., & Olmstead, M. (2003). Alcohol intoxication reduces impulsivity in the delay-discounting paradigm. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 38, 151–156. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agg041 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rachlin, H., & Green, L. (1972). Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 15–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rasmussen, E., Lawyer, S., & Reilly, W. (2010). Percent body fat is related to delay and probability discounting for food in humans. Behavioural Processes, 83, 23–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2009.09.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reimers, S., Maylor, E., Stewart, N., & Chater, N. (2009). Associations between a one-shot delay discounting measure and age, income, education and real-world impulsive behavior. Personality & Individual Differences, 47, 973–978. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.026 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans: Relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology, 17, 651–667. https://doi.org/10.1097/FBP.0b013e3280115f99 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reynolds, B., & Schiffbauer, R. (2005). Delay of gratification and delay discounting: A unifying feedback model of delay-related impulsive behavior. The Psychological Record, 55, 439–460. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395520 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richards, J., Zhang, L., Mitchell, S., & de Witt, H. (1999). Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: Effect of alcohol. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 121–143. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1999.71-121 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Robles, E., Huang, B., Simpson, P., & McMillan, D. (2011). Delay discounting, impulsiveness, and addiction severity in opioid-dependent patients. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 41, 354–362. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2011.05.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Saville, B., Gisbert, A., Kopp, J., & Telesco, C. (2010). Internet addiction and delay discounting in college students. The Psychological Record, 60, 273–286. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395707 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schweitzer, J., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1988). Self-control: Teaching tolerance for delay in impulsive children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 173–186. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1988.50-173 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shamosh, N., & Gray, J. (2008). Delay discounting and intelligence: A meta-analysis. Intelligence, 36, 289–305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2007.09.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheffer, C., MacKillop, J., McGeary, J., Landes, R., Carter, L., Yi, R., … Bickel, W. (2012). Delay discounting, locus of control, and cognitive impulsiveness independently predict tobacco dependence treatment outcomes in a highly dependent, lower socioeconomic group of smokers. American Journal on Addictions, 21, 221–232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1521-0391.2012.00224.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silverman, I. (2003). Gender differences in delay of gratification: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 49, 451–463. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stein, J., & Madden, G. (2013). Delay discounting and drug abuse: Empirical, conceptual, and methodological considerations. In J. MacKillopH. de WitEds., The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of addiction psychopharmacology (pp. 165–208). Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118384404.ch7 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stieg, M., & Dixon, M. (2007). Discounting of past and future rewards of Texas hold’em gamblers. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 8, 93–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2007.11434276 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swift, J., & Callahan, J. (2010). A delay discounting model of psychotherapy termination. International Journal of Behavioral Consultation & Therapy, 5, 278–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0100889 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, A. (2012). Are compulsive buyers impulsive? Evidence of poor response inhibition and delay discounting. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 794–806. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.025211 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wittmann, M., & Paulus, M. (2008). Decision-making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Youzhi, W., & Jing, L. (2009). A research on impulsivity and delay discounting differences between high and low procrastinators. Psychological Science, 32, 371–374. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar