Skip to main content
Research Article

Facial Likability and Smiling Enhance Cooperation, but Have No Direct Effect on Moralistic Punishment

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000338

Abstract. The present study serves to test how positive and negative appearance-based expectations affect cooperation and punishment. Participants played a prisoner’s dilemma game with partners who either cooperated or defected. Then they were given a costly punishment option: They could spend money to decrease the payoffs of their partners. Aggregated over trials, participants spent more money for punishing the defection of likable-looking and smiling partners compared to punishing the defection of unlikable-looking and nonsmiling partners, but only because participants were more likely to cooperate with likable-looking and smiling partners, which provided the participants with more opportunities for moralistic punishment. When expressed as a conditional probability, moralistic punishment did not differ as a function of the partners’ facial likability. Smiling had no effect on the probability of moralistic punishment, but punishment was milder for smiling in comparison to nonsmiling partners.

References

  • Bell, R., Buchner, A., Kroneisen, M. & Giang, T. (2012). On the flexibility of social source memory: A test of the emotional incongruity hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1512–1529. doi: 10.1037/a0028219 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bell, R., Buchner, A. & Musch, J. (2010). Enhanced old-new recognition and source memory for faces of cooperators and defectors in a social-dilemma game. Cognition, 117, 261–275. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.020 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bell, R., Giang, T., Mund, I. & Buchner, A. (2013). Memory for reputational trait information: Is social–emotional information processing less flexible in old age? Psychology and Aging, 28, 984–995. doi: 10.1037/a0034266 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bell, R., Sasse, J., Möller, M., Czernochowski, D., Mayr, S. & Buchner, A. (2016). Event-related potentials in response to cheating and cooperation in a social dilemma game. Psychophysiology, 53, 216–228. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12561 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Bernstein, F. (1925). Zusammenfassende Betrachtungen über die erblichen Blutstrukturen des Menschen.[Summarizing consideration on the inheritable blood structure of mankind] Zeitschrift für Abstammungs- und Vererbungslehre, 37, 237–270. doi: 10.1007/BF01763333 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, W. M. & Moore, C. (2002). Smile asymmetries and reputation as reliable indicators of likelihood to cooperate: An evolutionary analysis. In S. P. ShohovEd., Advances in psychology research (pp. 59–78). Huntington, NY: Nova Science. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, J. (2007). The demand for punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 62, 522–542. doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2005.05.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cinyabuguma, M., Page, T. & Putterman, L. (2006). Can second-order punishment deter perverse punishment? Experimental Economics, 9, 265–279. doi: 10.1007/s10683-006-9127-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • DeScioli, P. & Kurzban, R. (2009). The Alliance Hypothesis for Human Friendship. PLoS One, 4, e5802. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005802 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eckel, C. C. & Wilson, R. K. (2003). The human face of game theory: Trust and reciprocity in sequential games. In E. OstromJ. M. WalkerEds., Trust and reciprocity: Interdisciplinary lessons from experimental research (pp. 245–274). New York, NY: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V. & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1508–1519. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Erdfelder, E., Auer, T.-S., Hilbig, B. E., Aßfalg, A., Moshagen, M. & Nadarevic, L. (2009). Multinomial processing tree models. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 108–124. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.3.108 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Falk, A., Fehr, E. & Fischbacher, U. (2005). Driving forces behind informal sanctions. Econometrica, 73, 2017–2030. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0262.2005.00644.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fehr, E. & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137–140. doi: 10.1038/415137a First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Giang, T., Bell, R. & Buchner, A. (2012). Does facial resemblance enhance cooperation? PLoS One, 7, e47809. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047809 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Goldenthal, P., Johnston, R. & Kraut, R. (1981). Smiling, appeasement, and the silent bared-teeth display. Ethology and Sociobiology, 2, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/0162-3095(81)90025-X First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herrmann, B., Thöni, C. & Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319, 1362–1367. doi: 10.1126/science.1153808 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Hilbig, B. E., Erdfelder, E. & Pohl, R. F. (2010). One-reason decision making unveiled: A measurement model of the recognition heuristic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 123–134. doi: 10.1037/a0017518 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Klauer, K., Stahl, C. & Erdfelder, E. (2007). The abstract selection task: New data and an almost comprehensive model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 680–703. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.680 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kroneisen, M. & Bell, R. (2013). Sex, cheating, and disgust: Enhanced source memory for trait information that violates gender stereotypes. Memory, 21, 167–181. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.713971 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Lockard, J. S., Fahrenbruch, C. E., Smith, J. L. & Morgan, C. J. (1977). Smiling and laughter: Different phyletic origins? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 10, 183–186. doi: 10.3758/BF03329317 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ma, Q., Hu, Y., Jiang, S. & Meng, L. (2015). The undermining effect of facial attractiveness on brain responses to fairness in the ultimatum game: an ERP study. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00077 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mackey, W. (1976). Parameters of the Smile as a Social Signal. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 129, 125–130. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1976.10534019 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Minear, M. & Park, D. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 630–633. doi: 10.3758/BF03206543 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Moshagen, M. (2010). multiTree: A computer program for the analysis of multinomial processing tree models. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 42–54. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.1.42 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Moshagen, M., Hilbig, B. E. & Musch, J. (2011). Defection in the dark? A randomized-response investigation of cooperativeness in social dilemma games. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 638–644. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.793 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mussel, P., Göritz, A. S. & Hewig, J. (2013). The value of a smile: Facial expression affects ultimatum-game responses. Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 381–385. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mussel, P., Hewig, J., Allen, J., Coles, M. & Miltner, W. (2014). Smiling faces, sometimes they don’t tell the truth: Facial expression in the ultimatum game impacts decision making and event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 51, 358–363. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12184 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Nikiforakis, N. (2008). Punishment and counter-punishment in public good games: Can we really govern ourselves? Journal of Public Economics, 92, 91–112. doi: 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2007.04.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Preuschoft, S. (1992). “Laughter” and “smile” in Barbary Macaques (Macaca sylvanus). Ethology, 91, 220–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1992.tb00864.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Riefer, D. M. & Batchelder, W. H. (1988). Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes. Psychological Review, 95, 318–339. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.95.3.318 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmitt, M., Gollwitzer, M., Maes, J. & Arbach, D. (2005). Justice sensitivity: Assessment and location in the personality space. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, 202–211. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.21.3.202 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Solnick, S. & Schweitzer, M. (1999). The influence of physical attractiveness and gender on ultimatum game decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79, 199–215. doi: 10.1006/obhd.1999.2843 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Stahl, C. & Klauer, K. (2007). HMMTree: A computer program for latent-class hierarchical multinomial processing tree models. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 267–273. doi: 10.3758/BF03193157 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Todorov, A. (2008). Evaluating faces on trustworthiness. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 208–224. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.012 First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Willis, J. & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Wilson, R. K. & Eckel, C. C. (2006). Judging a book by its cover: Beauty and expectations in the trust game. Political Research Quarterly, 59, 189–202. doi: 10.1177/106591290605900202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar