Skip to main content
Original Article

Smiling After Thinking Increases Reliance on Thoughts

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000131

The present research examines the impact of smiling on attitude change. Participants were first exposed to a story that elicited mostly positive thoughts (about an employee’s good day at work) or negative thoughts (about an employee’s bad day at work). After writing down their thoughts, participants were asked to hold a pen with their teeth (smile) or with their lips (control). Finally, all participants reported the extent to which they liked the story. In line with the self-validation hypothesis, we predicted and found that the effect of the initial thought direction induction on story evaluations was greater for smiling than control participants. These results conceptually replicate those obtained in previous research on embodiment (i.e., more favorable evaluations of stories when smiling; Strack, Martin, & Stepper, 1988) when participants had positive thoughts, but showed the opposite pattern of results (less favorable evaluations for smiling) for negative thoughts.

References

  • Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62). New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bless, H. , Böhner, G. , Schwarz, N. , Strack, F. (1990). Mood and persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 331–345. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Briñol, P. , & Petty, R. E. (2003). Overt head movements and persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1123–1139. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Briñol, P. , & Petty, R. E. (2008). Embodied persuasion: Fundamental processes by which bodily responses can impact attitudes. In G. R. Semin, E. R. Smith, (Eds.), Embodiment grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 184–208). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Briñol, P. , Petty, R. E. , Barden, J. (2007). Happiness versus sadness as determinants of thought confidence in persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 711–727. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Briñol, P. , Petty, R. E. , Wagner, B. C. (2012). Embodied validation: Our body can change and also validate our thoughts. In P. Briñol, K. G. DeMarree, (Eds.), Social metacognition (pp. 219–240). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T. , & Petty, R. E. (1981). Social psychological procedures for cognitive response assessment: The thought listing technique. In T. Merluzzi, C. Glass, M. Genest, (Eds.), Cognitive assessment (pp. 309–342). New York, NY: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Clore, G. L. , Huntsinger, J. R. (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 393–399. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dunn, B. D. , Galton, H. C. , Morgan, R. , Evans, D. , Oiver, C. , Meyer, M. , Dalgleish, T. (2010). Listening to your heart: How interception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychological Science, 21, 1835–1844. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Effron, D. A. , Niedenthal, P. M. , Gil, S. , Droit-Volet, S. (2006). Embodied temporal perception of emotion. Emotion, 6, 1–9. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ekman, P. , Davidson, R. J. , Friesen, W. V. (1990). The Duchenne smile: Emotional expression and brain physiology II. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 342–353. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Förster, J. , & Strack, F. (1996). The influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: A case of conceptual-motor compatibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 421–430. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fredrickson, B. L. , & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313–332. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huntsinger, J. R. , & Clore, G. L. (2012). Emotion and meta-cognition. In P. Briñol, K. DeMarree, (Eds.), Social meta-cognition (pp. 199–218). New York, NY: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Keltner, D. , Anderson, C. (2000). Saving face for Darwin: The functions and uses of embarrassment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(6), 187–192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Laird, J. D. , Bresler, C. (1992). The process of emotional experience: A self-perception theory. In M. S. Clard, (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (pp. 213–234). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mackie, D. M. , & Worth, L. T. (1989). Cognitive deficits and the mediation of positive affect in persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 27–40. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, L. L. , Ward, D. W. , Achee, J. W. , Wyer, R. S. (1993). Mood as input: People have to interpret the motivational implications of their moods. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 317–326. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martinie, M. A. , & Fointiat, V. (2006). Self-esteem, trivialization, and attitude change. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 65, 221–225. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, R. , Förster, J. , Strack, F. (2003). Motor compatibility: The bi-directional link between behavior and evaluation. In J. Musch, K. C. Klauer, (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 371–391). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316, 1002–1005. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. , Briñol, P. , Tormala, Z. L. (2002). Thought confidence as a determinant of persuasion: The self-validation hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 722–741. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarz, N. , Bless, H. , & Bohner, G. (1991). Mood and persuasion: Affective status influence the processing of persuasive communications. In M. Zanna, (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 161–197). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schwarz, N. , Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simon, L. , Greenberg, J. , Brehm, J. (1995). Trivialization: The forgotten mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 247–260. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, C. A. , Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Soussignan, R. (2002). Duchenne smile, emotional experience, and autonomic reactivity: A test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Emotion, 2, 52–74. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stepper, S. , Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 211–220. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, F. , Martin, L. , & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tiedens, L. Z. , & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 973–988. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wegener, D. T. , Petty, R. E. , Smith, S. M. (1995). Positive mood can increase or decrease message scrutiny: The hedonic contingency view of mood and message processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 5–15. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar