Skip to main content

Functional Equivalence of the Color Red and Enacted Avoidance Behavior?

Replication and Empirical Integration

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000245

Abstract. Research has shown that subtle stimuli and action can elicit approach and avoidance motivational states. In separate literatures, both the color red and enacting avoidance behavior have been hypothesized to evoke avoidance motivation. The purpose of the present research was to both replicate and empirically integrate prior work on red and enacted avoidance behavior. This was done by testing them together within the same paradigms in two experiments, one on anagram performance and the other on local-relative-to-global processing. Both experiments replicated prior research in each literature, with red and enacted avoidance behavior producing comparable effects across both experiments. Implications of the findings for the two literatures are discussed.

References

  • Bertrams, A., Baumeister, R. F., Englert, C. & Furley, P. (2015). Ego depletion in color priming research self-control strength moderates the detrimental effect of red on cognitive test performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 311–322. doi: 10.1177/0146167214564968 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Bertnson, G. G. & Crites, S. L. (1996). Social neuroscience: Principles of psychophysiological arousal and response. In E. T. HigginsA. W. KruglanksiEds., Social Psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 72–101). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J. T. & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 191–214. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.191 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chen, M. & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 215–224. doi: 10.1177/0146167299025002007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Derryberry, D. & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. M. NiedenthalS. KitayamaEds., Heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167–196). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201. doi: 10.1037/h0047707 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J. & Maier, M. A. (2014). Color psychology: Effects of perceiving color on psychological functioning in humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 95–120. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115035 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., Moller, A. C., Friedman, R. & Meinhardt, J. (2007). Color and psychological functioning: The effect of red on performance attainment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 154–168. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.136.1.154 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fayant, M. P., Muller, D., Nurra, C., Alexopoulos, T. & Palluel-Germain, R. (2011). Moving forward is not only a metaphor: Approach and avoidance leads to self-evaluative assimilation and contrast. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 241–245. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Förster, J., Friedman, R. S., Özelsel, A. & Denzler, M. (2006). Enactment of approach and avoidance behavior influences the scope of perceptual and conceptual attention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 133–146. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.02.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, R. S. & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1001–1013. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.1001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friedman, R. S. & Förster, J. (2010). Implicit affective cues and attentional tuning: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 875–893. doi: 10.1037/a0020495 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gasper, K. (2004). Do you see what I see? Affect and visual information processing. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 405–421. doi: 10.1080/02699930341000068 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Genschow, O., Reutner, L. & Wänke, M. (2012). The color red reduces snack food and soft drink intake. Appetite, 58, 699–702. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.023 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gilhooly, K. J. (1978). Bigram statistics for 205 five-letter words having single-solution anagrams. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 10, 389–392. doi: 10.3758/BF03205158 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gino, F. & Margolis, J. D. (2011). Bringing ethics into focus: How regulatory focus and risk preferences influence (un)ethicial behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115, 145–156. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.01.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gnambs, T., Appel, M. & Batinic, B. (2010). Color red in web-based knowledge testing. Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 1625–1631. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2010.06.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenlees, I. A., Eynon, M. & Thelwell, R. C. (2013). Color of soccer goalkeepers’ uniforms influences the outcome of penalty kicks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 116, 1–10. doi: 10.1080/02640410701744446 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Houtman, F. & Notebaert, W. (2013). Blinded by an error. Cognition, 128, 228–236. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ishihara, S. (1972). Tests for Colour Blindness, Tokyo, Japan: Kanehara Shuppan. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Janoff-Bulman, R., Sheikh, S. & Hepp, S. (2009). Proscriptive versus prescriptive morality: Two faces of moral regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 521–537. doi: 10.1037/a0013779 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kimchi, R. & Palmer, S. E. (1982). Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 521–535. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.521 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kliger, D. & Gilad, D. (2012). Red light, green light: Color priming in financial decisions. Journal of Socio-Economics, 41, 738–745. doi: 10.1016/j.socec.2012.07.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krieglmeyer, R., De Houwer, J. & Deutsch, R. (2013). On the nature of automatically triggered approach-avoidance behavior. Emotion Review, 5, 280–284. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477501 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lang, P. J. (1995). The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50, 372–385. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.50.5.372 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maier, M. A., Elliot, A. J. & Lichtenfeld, S. (2008). Mediation of the negative effect of red on intellectual performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1530–1540. doi: 10.1177/0146167208323104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Memmert, D., Hüttermann, S. & Orliczek, J. (2013). Decide like Lionel Messi! The impact of regulatory focus on divergent thinking in sports. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 163–167. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12159 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Murphy, S. T. & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.64.5.723 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nussinson, R., Häfner, M., Seibt, B., Strack, F. & Trope, Y. (2012). Approach/avoidance orientations affect self-construal and identification with in-group. Self and Identity, 11, 255–272. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2011.559044 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rook, L. & van Knippenberg, D. (2011). Creativity and imitation: Effects of regulatory focus and creative exemplar quality. Creativity Research Journal, 23, 346–356. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2011.621844 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Roskes, M., De Dreu, C. K. W. & Nijstad, B. A. (2012). Necessity is the mother of invention: Avoidance motivation stimulates creativity through cognitive effort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 242–256. doi: 10.1037/a0028442 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rutchick, A. M., Slepian, M. L. & Ferris, B. D. (2010). The pen is mightier than the word: Object priming of evaluative standards. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 704–708. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.753 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. R. JonesEd., Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 1–42). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shavit, T., Rosenboim, M. & Cohen, C. (2013). Does the color of feedback affect investment decisions?. International Journal of Applied Behavioral Economics, 2, 15–26. doi: 10.4018/ijabe.2013070102 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Shi, J., Zhang, C. & Jiang, F. (2015). Does red undermine individuals’ intellectual performance? A test in China. International Journal of Psychology, 50, 81–84. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12076 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spielberger, C. D. & Vagg, P. R. (1995). Test anxiety: A transactional process model. In C. D. SpielbergerP. R. VaggEds., Test anxiety: Theory, assessment, and treatment. Series in clinical and community psychology (pp. 3–14). Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics, Boston, MA: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Tanaka, A. & Tokuno, Y. (2011). The effect of the color red on avoidance motivation. Social Behavior and Personality, 39, 287–288. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2011.39.2.287 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ten Velden, F. S., Baas, M., Shalvi, S., Preenan, P. T. Y. & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2012). In competitive interaction displays of red increase actors’ competitive approach and perceivers’ withdrawal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 1205–1208. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.04.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Steenbergen, H., Band, G. P. H. & Hommel, B. (2011). Threat but not arousal narrows attention: Evidence from pupil dilation and saccade control. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00281 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhang, T. & Han, B. (2014). Experience reverses the red effect among Chinese stockbrokers. PLoS One, 9, e89193. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0089193 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar