Skip to main content
Original Article

Demand to Act and Use of Mental Contrasting

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000353

Abstract. Mentally contrasting a desired future with present reality fosters selective goal pursuit: People pursue feasible desired futures and let go from unfeasible ones. We investigated whether people spontaneously use mental contrasting when the demand to act toward their desired future is high. Study 1 provided correlational evidence: The participants who planned to act most immediately were also those who used mental contrasting. Studies 2 and 3 added experimental evidence: Imagining an immediate (vs. no immediate) action and being confronted with the opportunity to perform an instrumental (vs. noninstrumental) action, respectively, led participants to mentally contrast. The findings have theoretical implications by suggesting that people initiate mental contrasting as a problem-solving strategy; they have applied implications for interventions teaching mental contrasting.

References

  • Achtziger, A., Fehr, T., Oettingen, G., Gollwitzer, P. M. & Rockstroh, B. (2009). Strategies of intention formation are reflected in continuous MEG activity. Social Neuroscience, 4, 11–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910801925350 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Adriaanse, M. A., de Ridder, D. T. D. & Voorneman, I. (2013). Improving diabetes self-management by mental contrasting. Psychology and Health, 28, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2012.660154 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cao, L. (2012). Examining “active” procrastination from a self-regulated learning perspective. Educational Psychology, 32, 515–545. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2012.663722 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Chu, A. H. C. & Choi, J. N. (2005). Rethinking procrastination: Positive effects of “active” procrastination behavior on attitudes and performance. Journal of Social Psychology, 14, 245–264. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.145.3.245-264 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Darley, J. M. & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 377–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025589 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Witt Huberts, J. C., Evers, C. & De Ridder, D. T. D. (2014). “Because I am worth it”: A theoretical framework and empirical review of a justification-based account of self-regulation failure. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18, 119–138. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868313507533 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dweck, C. S. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality and development (1st ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G. & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S. & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not into temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 296–309. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. (2004). Social beings: A core motives approach to social psychology. New York, NY: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Influences of information and motivation on attention and interpretation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 1–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60317-2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fiske, S. T. & Taylor, S. E. (1984). Social cognition (1st ed.). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P. M. & Trope, Y. (2004). The influence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and guiding others’ self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 739–752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.04.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fujita, K. & Carnevale, J. J. (2012). Transcending temptation through abstraction: The role of construal level in self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 248–252. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412449169 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fujita, K. & Han, H. A. (2009). The effect of construal levels on evaluative associations in self-control conflicts. Psychological Science, 20, 799–804. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02372.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N. & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 351–367. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.351 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, A., Oettingen, G., Kirby, T., Duckworth, A. & Mayer, D. (2011). Mental contrasting facilitates academic performance in school children. Motivation and Emotion, 35, 403–412. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9222-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. The American Psychologist, 54, 493–503. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harriott, J. & Ferrari, J. R. (1996). Prevalence of procrastination among samples of adults. Psychological Reports, 78, 611–616. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1996.78.2.611 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Howell, D. C. (2007). Statistical methods for psychology (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Inzlicht, M. & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 450–463. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612454134 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kappes, A. & Oettingen, G. (2014). The emergence of goal pursuit: Mental contrasting connects future and reality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 25–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.03.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kappes, A., Oettingen, G. & Pak, H. P. (2012). Mental contrasting and the self-regulation of responding to negative feedback. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 845–857. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167212446833 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kappes, A., Singmann, H. & Oettingen, G. (2012). Mental contrasting instigates goal pursuit by linking obstacles of reality with instrumental behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 811–818. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kappes, A., Wendt, M., Reinelt, T. & Oettingen, G. (2013). Mental contrasting changes the meaning of reality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 797–810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kappes, H. B., Oettingen, G., Mayer, D. & Maglio, S. (2011). Sad mood promotes self-initiated mental contrasting of future and reality. Emotion, 11, 1206–1222. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023983 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0076171 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kovaleva, A., Beierlein, C., Kemper, C. J. & Rammstedt, B. (2012). Eine Kurzskala zur Messung von Kontrollüberzeugung: Die Skala Internale-Externale-Kontrollüberzeugung-4 (IE-4) [A brief scale to measure locus of control: The internal-external locus of control scale-4 (IE-4)]. Köln, Germany: GESIS. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R., Chun, W. Y. & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal-systems. In M. P. ZannaEd., Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). New York, NY: Academic Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McCrea, S. M., Liberman, N., Trope, Y. & Sherman, S. J. (2008). Construal level and procrastination. Psychological Science, 19, 1308–1314. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02240.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y. & Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2658056 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M. & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M., Shmueli, D. & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 524–537. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Myrseth, K. O. R., Fishbach, A. & Trope, Y. (2009). Counteractive self-control: When making temptation available makes temptation less tempting. Psychological Science, 20, 159–163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02268.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oettingen, G. (2000). Expectancy effects on behavior depend on self-regulatory thought. Social Cognition, 18, 101–129. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2000.18.2.101 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oettingen, G., Mayer, D. & Brinkmann, B. (2010). Mental contrasting of future and reality: Managing the demands of everyday life in health care professionals. Journal of Personnel Psychology, 9, 138–144. https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000018 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Sevincer, A. T., Stephens, E. J., Pak, H. & Hagenah, M. (2009). Mental contrasting and goal commitment: The mediating role of energization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 608–622. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208330856 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oettingen, G., Pak, H. & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation of goal setting: Turning free fantasies about the future into binding goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 736–753. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.736 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oettingen, G. & Sevincer, A. T. (2018). Fantasy about the future as friend and foe. In G. OettingenA. T. SevincerP. M. GollwitzerEds., The psychology of thinking about the future (pp. 127–149). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petty, R. E. & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The elaboration likelihood model: Current status and controversies. In S. ChaikenY. TropeEds., Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 41–72). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pintrich, P. R. & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Richter, M., Gendolla, G. H. E. & Wright, R. A. (2016). Three decades of research on motivational intensity theory: What we have learned about effort and what we still don’t know. Advances in Motivation Science, 3, 149–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.adms.2016.02.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sevincer, A. T., Busatta, P. D. & Oettingen, G. (2014). Mental contrasting and transfer of energization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 139–152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213507088 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sevincer, A. T., Kluge, L. & Oettingen, G. (2014). Implicit theories and motivational focus: Desired future versus present reality. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 36–46. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9359-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sevincer, A. T., Mehl, P. J. & Oettingen, G. (2017). Well self-regulated people use mental contrasting. Social Psychology, 48, 348–364. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000322 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Sevincer, A. T. & Oettingen, G. (2015). Future thought and the self-regulation of energization. In G. H. E. GendollaM. TopsS. KooleEds., Biobehavioral approaches to self-regulation (pp. 315–329). New York, NY: Springer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sevincer, A. T. & Oettingen, G. (2013). Spontaneous mental contrasting and selective goal pursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1240–1254. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213492428 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sevincer, A. T., Schlier, B. & Oettingen, G. (2015). Mental contrasting and ego-depletion. Motivation and Emotion, 39, 876–891. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9508-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheeran, P., Harris, P., Vaughan, J., Oettingen, G. & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2013). Gone exercising: Mental contrasting promotes physical activity among overweight, middle-aged, low-SES fishermen. Health Psychology, 32, 802–809. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029293 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trope, Y. & Fishbach, A. (2000). Counteractive self-control in overcoming temptation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 493–506. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.4.493 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117, 440–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tukey, J. W. (1962). The future of data analysis. Annals of Mathematical Statistics, 33, 1–67. https://doi.org/10.1214/aoms/1177704711 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vallacher, R. R. & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 660–671. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.4.660 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A. & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm’s theory of motivation as a model of effort and cardiovascular response. In P. M. GollwitzerJ. A. BarghEds., The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 424–453). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar