Skip to main content
Original Article

When and Why Do Liberals and Conservatives Think Alike?

An Investigation into Need for Cognitive Closure, the Binding Moral Foundations, and Political Perception

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000354

Abstract. Research on moral foundations theory has found that liberals typically favor the individualizing foundations (i.e., concern for the individual) but typically oppose the binding foundations (i.e., concern for the group). We propose that need for cognitive closure (NFC) can explain when liberals will favor the binding foundations. In two studies, we found that liberals in Italy and the United States were more likely to endorse the binding foundations when they had high NFC. Overall, these results suggest that researchers should strive to understand the interaction between individuals’ epistemic goals and their political orientations in order to accurately predict how they will perceive political issues.

References

  • Bobbio, A., Nencini, A. & Sarrica, M. (2011). Il moral foundations questionnaire: Analisi della struttura fattoriale della versione italiana [The moral foundations questionnaire: Structural factor analyses of the Italian version]. Giornale di Psicologia, 5, 7–18. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bull, A. C. (2018, February 19). The intricacies of coalition-making in the 2018 Italian election. Speri.comment. [Blog post]. Retrieved from http://speri.dept.shef.ac.uk/2018/02/19/the-intricacies-of-coalition-making-in-the-2018-italian-electoral-campaign/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Conover, P. J. & Feldman, S. (1981). The origins and meaning of liberal/conservative self-identifications. American Journal of Political Science, 25, 617–645. https://doi.org/10.2307/2110756 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cornwell, J. F. & Higgins, E. T. (2014). Locomotion concerns with moral usefulness: When liberals endorse conservative binding moral foundations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 109–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.09.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dijksterhuis, A., van Knippenberg, A., Kruglanski, A. W. & Schaper, C. (1996). Motivated social cognition: Need for closure effects on memory and judgment. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychology, 32, 254–270. https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1996.0012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Duarte, J. L., Crawford, J. T., Stern, C., Haidt, J., Jussim, L. & Tetlock, P. E. (2014). Political diversity will improve social psychological science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e130. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Fabbrini, S. (2009). The transformation of Italian democracy. Bulletin of Italian Politics, 1, 29–47. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Federico, C. M., Deason, G. & Fisher, E. L. (2012). Ideological asymmetry in the relationship between epistemic motivation and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 381–398. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029063 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Federico, C. M., Ekstrom, P., Tagar, M. R. & Williams, A. L. (2016). Epistemic motivation and the structure of moral intuition: Dispositional need for closure as a predictor of individualizing. European Journal of Personality, 30, 227–239. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2055 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J. & Nosek, B. A. (2008). The moral foundations questionnaire. Retrieved from http://www.MoralFoundations.org First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J. & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 1029. https://doi.org/0.1037/a0015141 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gray, K. & Schein, C. (2012). Two minds vs. two philosophies: Mind perception defines morality and dissolves the debate between deontology and utilitarianism. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-012-0112-5 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gray, K., Schein, C. & Ward, A. F. (2014). The myth of harmless wrongs in moral cognition: Automatic dyadic completion from sin to suffering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1600–1615. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036149 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hansen, L. A. (2010, November 7). Animal research: Groupthink in both camps. Chronicle of Higher Education. Retrieved from https://www.chronicle.com First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hardin, C. D. & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. In E. T. HigginsEd., Handbook of motivation and cognition, Vol. 3: The interpersonal context (pp. 28–84). New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W. & Glaser, J. (2003). Exceptions that prove the rule-using a theory of motivated social cognition to account for ideological incongruities and political anomalies: Reply to Greenberg and Jonas (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 383–393. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.383 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W. & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koleva, S. P., Graham, J., Iyer, R., Ditto, P. H. & Haidt, J. (2012). Tracing the threads: How five moral concerns (especially Purity) help explain culture war attitudes. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(2), 184–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.01.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational bases. New York, NY: Plenum Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Pierro, A., Higgins, E. T. & Capozza, D. (2007). “On the move” or “staying put”: Locomotion, need for closure, and reactions to organizational change. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 1305–1340. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00214.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Thompson, E. P., Higgins, E. T., Atash, M. N., Pierro, A., Shah, J. Y. & Spiegel, S. (2000). To “do the right thing” or to “just do it”: Locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 793–815. https://doi.org/0.1037/0022-3514.79.5.793 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kruglanski, A. W. & Webster, D. M. (1996). Motivated closing of the mind: “Seizing” and “freezing”. Psychological Review, 103, 263–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.2.263 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Parent, M. C. (2013). Handling item-level missing data: Simpler is just as good. The Counseling Psychologist, 41, 568–600. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000012445176 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pierro, A. & Kruglanski, A. W. (2005). Revised need for cognitive closure scale Unpublished manuscript. University of Rome “La Sapienza”. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roets, A., Kruglanski, A. W., Kossowska, M., Pierro, A. & Hong, Y. Y. (2015). The motivated gatekeeper of our minds: New directions in need for closure theory and research. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 52, 221–283. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.01.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87(2), 245–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.87.2.245 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strojny, P., Kossoswka, M. & Strojny, A. (2016). Search for expectancy inconsistent information reduces uncertainty better: The role of cognitive capacity. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 395–410. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00395 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Webster, D. M. & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wolsko, C., Ariceaga, H. & Seiden, J. (2016). Red, white, and blue enough to be green: Effects of moral framing on climate change attitudes and conservation behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 7–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.02.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wood, T. J. & Oliver, J. E. (2012). Towards a more reliable implementation of ideology in measures of mass opinion. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76, 636–662. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfs045 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yancey, G., Reimer, S. & O’Connell, J. (2015). How academics view conservative protestants. Sociology of Religion, 76, 315–336. https://doi.org/10.1093/socrel/srv027 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar