Skip to main content
Original Article

Ego Depletion May Disappear by 2020

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000375

Abstract. Ego depletion has been successfully replicated in hundreds of studies. Yet the most recent large-scale Registered Replication Reports (RRR), comprising thousands of participants, have yielded disappointingly small effects, sometimes even failing to reach statistical significance. Although these results may seem surprising, in the present article I suggest that they are perfectly consistent with a long-term decline in the size of the depletion effects that can be traced back to at least 10 years ago, well before any of the RRR on ego depletion were conceived. The decline seems to be at least partly due to a parallel trend toward publishing better and less biased research.

References

  • Allen, C. P. G., & Mehler, D. M. A. (2018). Open Science challenges, benefits and tips in early career and beyond. Unpublished manuscript. Available at https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3czyt First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F. (2016). Charting the future of social psychology on stormy seas: Winners, losers, and recommendations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 153–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2016.02.003 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Misguided effort with elusive implications. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 574–575. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616652878 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Forsell, E., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., … Wu, H. (2016). Evaluating replicability of laboratory experiments in economics. Science, 351, 1433–1436. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf0918 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., … Wu, H. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 637–644. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, E. C., Kofler, L. M., Forster, D. E., & McCullough, M. E. (2015). A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 796–815. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000083 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2013). Is ego depletion too incredible? Evidence for the overestimation of the depletion effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 683–684. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13000952 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2014). Publication bias and the limited strength model of self-control: Has the evidence for ego depletion been overestimated? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 823. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00823 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2018). A simple, principled approach to combining evidence from meta-analysis and high-quality replications. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1, 174–185. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245918756858 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 181–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dang, J. (2016). Commentary: “A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect”. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1155. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01155 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dang, J. (2018). An updated meta-analysis of the ego depletion effect. Psychological Research, 82, 645–651. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0862-x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dang, J., Liu, Y., Liu, X., & Mao, L. (2017). The ego could be depleted, providing initial exertion is depleting. Social Psychology, 48, 242–245. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000308 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Dijksterhuis, A. (2018). Reflection on the professor priming replication report. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 295–296. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618755705 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Drummond, A., & Philipp, M. C. (2017). Commentary: “Misguided effort with elusive implications” and “A multi-lab pre-registered replication of the ego depletion effect”. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 273. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00273 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ebersole, C. R., Atherton, O. E., Belanger, A. L., Skulborstad, H. M., Allen, J. M., Banks, J. B., … Nosek, B. A. (2016). Many Labs 3: Evaluating participant pool quality across the academic semester via replication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 68–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.10.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Emmerling, F., Martijn, C., Alberts, H. J. E. M., Thomson, A. C., David, B., Kessler, D., … Sack, A. T. (2017). The (non-)replicability of regulatory resource depletion: A field report employing non-invasive brain stimulation. PLoS One, 12, e0174331. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174331 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Francis, G. (2012). Too good to be true: Publication bias in two prominent studies from experimental psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 151–156. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0227-9 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Franco, A., Malhotra, N., & Simonovits, G. (2014). Publication bias in the social sciences: Unlocking the file drawer. Science, 345, 1502–1505. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1255484 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Friese, M., Loschelder, D. D., Gieseler, K., Frankenbach, J., & Inzlicht, M. (in press). Is ego depletion real? An analysis of arguments. Personality and Social Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868318762183 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Garrison, K. E., Finley, A. J., & Schmeichel, B. J. (in press). Ego depletion reduces attention control: Evidence from two high-powered preregistered experiments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218796473 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gehr, B. T., Weiss, C., & Porzsolt, F. (2006). The fading of reported effectiveness: A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 6, 25. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-6-25 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gilbert, D. T., King, G., Pettigrew, S., & Wilson, T. D. (2016). Comment on “Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science”. Science, 351, 1037b. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad7243 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Alberts, H., Angonno, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A., … Zwienenberg, M. (2016). A multi-lab pre-registered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 546–573. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616652873 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 495–525. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019486 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hawkins, R. X. D., Smith, E. N., Au, C., Arias, J. M., Catapano, R., Hermann, E., … Frank, M. C. (2018). Improving the replicability of psychological science through pedagogy. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1, 7–18. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245917740427 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ioannidis, J. P. A. (1998). Effect of the statistical significance of results on the time to completion and publication of randomized efficacy trials. Journal of the American Medical Association, 279, 281–286. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.279.4.281 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2008). Why most discovered true associations are inflated. Epidemiology, 19, 640–648. https://doi.org/10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818131e7 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jennions, M. D., & Moller, A. P. (2002). Relationships fade with time: A meta-analysis of temporal trends in publication in ecology and evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 269, 43–48. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1832 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Johnsen, T. J., & Friborg, O. (2015). The effects of cognitive behavioral therapy as an anti-depressive treatment is falling: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 14, 747–768. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000015 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaplan, R. M., & Irvin, V. L. (2015). Likelihood of null effects on large NHLBI clinical trials has increased over time. PLoS One, 10, e0132382. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0132382 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Klein, R. A., Ratliff, K. A., Vianello, M., Adams, R. B. Jr., Bahnik, S., Bernstein, M. J., … Nosek, B. A. (2014). Investigating variation in replicability: A “many labs” replication project. Social Psychology, 45, 142–152. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000178 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Lehrer, J. (2011, December 13). The truth wears off: Is there something wrong with the scientific method? New Yorker Magazine, 52–56. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lurquin, J. H., Michaelson, L. E., Barker, J. E., Gustavson, D. E., von Bastian, C. C., Carruth, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2016). No evidence of the ego-depletion effect across task characteristics and individual differences: A pre-registered study. PLoS One, 11, e0147770. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0147770 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morris, S. B., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods, 7, 105–125. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.105 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self- control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • O’Donnell, M., Nelson, L. D., Ackermann, E., Aczel, B., Akhtar, A., Aldrovandi, S., … Zrubka, M. (2018). Registered Replication Report: Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg (1998). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 268–294. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618755704 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349, aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Papageorgiou, S. N., Xavier, G. M., Cobourne, M. T., & Eliades, T. (2018). Registered trials report less beneficial treatment effects than unregistered ones: A meta-epidemiological study in orthodontics. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 100, 44–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2018.04.017 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pashler, H., & Wagenmakers, E. J. (2012). Editors’ introduction to the special section on replicability in psychological science: A crisis of confidence? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 528–530. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612465253 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pereira, T. V., Horwitz, R. I., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2012). Empirical evaluation of very large treatment effects of medical interventions. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 308, 1676–1684. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2012.13444 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Protzko, J. (2017, September 13). Kids these days. Retrieved from https://osf.io/j9tuz/ First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Protzko, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2017). Decline effects: Types, mechanisms, and personal reflections. In S. O. LilienfeldI. D. WaldmanEds., Psychological science under scrutiny: Recent challenges and proposed solutions (pp. 85–107). Chichester, UK: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rand, D. G. (2018). Non-naivety may reduce the effect of intuition manipulations. Nature Human Behavior, 2, 602. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0404-6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In A. H. BlackW. F. ProkasyEds., Classical conditioning II (pp. 64–99). New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638–641. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schooler, J. (2011). Unpublished results hide the decline effect. Nature, 470, 437. https://doi.org/10.1038/470437a First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simmons, L. W., Tomkins, J. L., Kotiaho, J. S., & Hunt, J. (1999). Fluctuating paradigm. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 266, 593–595. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1999.0677 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simons, D. J., Holcombe, A. O., & Spellman, B. A. (2014). An introduction to registered replication reports at Perspectives on Psychological Science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 552–555. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614543974 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simonsohn, U. (2014). No-way interactions. [Blog post]. Retrieved from http://datacolada.org/17 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Singh, R. K., & Göritz, A. S. (2018). Ego depletion does not interfere with working memory performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 538. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00538 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sripada, C., Kessler, D., & Jonides, J. (2014). Methylphenidate blocks effort-induced depletion of regulatory control in healthy volunteers. Psychological Science, 25, 1227–1234. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614526415 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Srivastava, S. [@hardsci] (2018, March 3). About to start – a symposium session on fresh* results from a multi-site ego depletion study #spsp2018 [Tweet]. Retrieved from https://twitter.com/hardsci/status/970015349499465729 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sterling, T. D. (1959). Publication decisions and their possible effects on inferences drawn from tests of significance – or vice versa. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 54, 30–34. 10.2307/2282137 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Stern, J. M., & Simes, R. J. (1997). Publication bias: Evidence of delayed publication in a cohort study of clinical research projects. British Medical Journal, 315, 640–645. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.640 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sterne, J. A. C., Sutton, A. J., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Terrin, N., Jones, D. R., Lau, J., … Higgins, J. P. (2011). Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 343, 4002. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d4002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, F. (2016). Reflection on the smiling registered replication report. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 929–930. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616674460 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trikalinos, T. A., Churchill, R., Ferri, M., Leucht, S., Tuunainen, A., Wahlbeck, K., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2004). Effect sizes in cumulative meta-analyses of mental health randomized trials evolved over time. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 57, 1124–1130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.02.018 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trikalinos, T. A., & Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Assessing the evolution of effect sizes over time. In H. R. RothsteinA. J. SuttonM. BorensteinEds., Publication bias and meta-analysis: Prevention, assessment and adjustments (pp. 241–259). New York, NY: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Vadillo, M. A., Gold, N., & Osman, M. (2018). Searching for the bottom of the ego well: Failure to uncover ego depletion in Many Labs 3. Royal Society Open Science, 5, 180390. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.180390 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vohs, K. D. (2015). Money priming can change people’s thoughts, feelings, motivations, and behaviors: An update on 10 years of experiments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, e86–e93. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000091 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wagenmakers, E.-J., Beek, T., Dijkhoff, L., Gronau, Q. F., Acosta, A., Adams, R. B. Jr., … Zwaan, R. A. (2016). Registered Replication Report: Strack, Martin, & Stepper (1988). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 917–928. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616674458 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yost, T. A. (2016). Meta-analytic and empirical estimates of the resource depletion effect size (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MI. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar