Abstract
Abstract. Agency and communion comprise the big two of social perception, with agency in general denoting goal orientation and communion orientation toward others. Both dimensions can be coded and detected in language through agentic and communal words, however, only agency through lexical classes, namely verbs. This research examines whether the link between semantic and lexical agency occurs in natural language, as past research was limited to pseudowords or single words. Using existing databases, Study 1 found that people rated verbs as more agentic than nouns. Using large textual datasets, Study 2 found verb use to be positively related to agentic – but not communal word – use. These findings contribute to the identification of agency in language and indicate its importance in social cognition.
References
2021). Navigating the social world: Toward an integrated framework for evaluating self, individuals, and groups. Psychological Review, 128(2), 290–314. 10.1037/rev0000262
(2008). Towards an operationalization of the fundamental dimensions of agency and communion: Trait content ratings in five countries considering valence and frequency of word occurrence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(7), 1202–1217. 10.1002/ejsp.575
(2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), Article
(5 . 10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.7512014). Communal and agentic content in social cognition: A dual perspective model. In M. P. ZannaJ. M. Olson (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 195–255). Academic Press.
(2018). The determinants of consciousness of human faces. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(3), Article
(3 . 10.1038/s41562-017-0266-32022). coreNLP: Wrappers Around Stanford CoreNLP Tools (0.4-3). https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=coreNLP
(1966). The duality of human existence: An essay on psychology and religion. Rand Mcnally
(2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 1–26. 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1
(2008). Adaptivity: From metabolism to behavior. Adaptive Behavior, 16(5), 325–344. 10.1177/1059712308093868
(2006). The Chicken or the Egg? A probabilistic analysis of English binomials. Language, 82(2), 233–278. 10.1353/lan.2006.0077
(2006). The extended argument dependency model: A neurocognitive approach to sentence comprehension across languages. Psychological Review, 113(4), 787–821. 10.1037/0033-295X.113.4.787
(2009). The role of prominence information in the real-time comprehension of transitive constructions: A cross-linguistic approach. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3(1), 19–58. 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00099.x
(2015). Did Shakespeare write double falsehood? Identifying individuals by creating psychological signatures with text analysis. Psychological Science, 26(5), 570–582. 10.1177/0956797614566658
(2016). A way with words: Using language for psychological science in the modern era. In C. V. Dimofte, C. P. Haugtvedt, & R. F. Yalch (Eds.), Consumer psychology in a social media world (pp. 222–236). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
(2010). Comparison focus in intergroup comparisons: Who we compare to whom influences who we see as powerful and agentic. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(10), 1424–1435. 10.1177/0146167210383581
(2013). The density of the Big Two: How are agency and communion structurally represented? Social Psychology, 44(2), 63–74. 10.1027/1864-9335/a000145
(2014). Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 904–911. 10.3758/s13428-013-0403-5
(2022). Gender bias in word embeddings: A comprehensive analysis of frequency, syntax, and semantics. Proceedings of the 2022 AAAI/ACM Conference on AI, Ethics, and Society (pp. 156–170). AAAI. 10.1145/3514094.3534162
(2008). Nomina sunt omina: On the inductive potential of nouns and adjectives in person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 839–859. 10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.839
(2014). How verb tense affects the construal of action: The simple past tense leads people into an abstract mindset. Psicologica: Revista De Metodologia Y Psicologia Experimental, 35(2), 209–223.
(2018). Power corrupts, but control does not: What stands behind the effects of holding high positions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(6), 944–957. 10.1177/0146167218757456
(2020). Closed and Open vocabulary approaches to text analysis: A review, quantitative comparison, and recommendations. PsyArXiv. 10.31234/osf.io/t52c6
(2020). tm—Text Mining Package, R Package Version 0.7-8. http://tm.r-forge.r-project.org/
(2020). Verb intergroup bias: Verbs are used more often in reference to in-groups than out-groups. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(6), 854–864. 10.1177/1948550619893957
(2018). Understanding dehumanization: The role of agency and communion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 77(2), 102–116. 10.1016/j.jesp.2018.04.003
(2021). Make it happen!”: Verbs as markers of agency increase message effectiveness. Social Psychology, 52(2), 75–89. 10.1027/1864-9335/a000435
(2017). Verbs as linguistic markers of agency: The social side of grammar. European Journal of Social Psychology, 47(5), 566–579. 10.1002/ejsp.2231
(2010). The social brain: Allowing humans to boldly go where no other species has been. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1537), 165–176. 10.1098/rstb.2009.0160
(2011). Evidence that gendered wording in job advertisements exists and sustains gender inequality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(1), Article
(1 . 10.1037/a00225301999). Carrot-Eaters and creature-believers: The effects of lexicalization on children’s inferences about social categories. Psychological Science, 10(6), 489–493. 10.1111/1467-9280.00194
(2022). List of English words. https://github.com/dwyl/english-words
(2013). Nouns cut slices: Effects of linguistic forms on intergroup bias. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 32(1), 62–83. 10.1177/0261927X12463209
(2011). When gentlemen are first and ladies are last: Effects of gender stereotypes on the order of romantic partners’ names. The British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(Pt 1), 21–35. 10.1348/014466610X486347
(1980). Transitivity in grammar and discourse. Language, 56(2), 251–299. 10.2307/413757
(2018). A rose by any other name? A subtle linguistic cue impacts anger and corresponding policy support in intractable conflict. Psychological Science, 29(6), 972–983. 10.1177/0956797618772823
(2017). Word order denotes relevance differences: The case of conjoined phrases with lexical gender. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(2), 262–279. 10.1037/pspi0000094
(2012). Can an agentic black woman get ahead? The impact of race and interpersonal dominance on perceptions of female leaders. Psychological Science, 23(4), 354–358. 10.1177/0956797611428079
(2022). The sense of agency in joint action: An integrative review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(4), 1089–1117. 10.3758/s13423-021-02051-3
(2022). Reconciling female agentic advantage and disadvantage with the CADDIS measure of agency. Journal of Applied Psychology, 107(12), 2115–2148. 10.1037/apl0000550
(2009). Gender and letters of recommendation for academia: Agentic and communal differences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(6), 1591–1599. 10.1037/a0016539
(2014). The Stanford CoreNLP natural language processing toolkit. Proceedings of 52nd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics: System Demonstrations (pp. 55–60). ACL. 10.3115/v1/P14-5010
(1993). Building a large annotated corpus of English: The Penn Treebank. Computational Linguistics, 19(2), 313–330. https://aclanthology.org/J93-2004
(2018). On minimal autonomous agency: Natural and artificial. Complex Systems, 27(3), 289–313. 10.25088/ComplexSystems.27.3.289
(2014). Nouns, verbs, objects, actions, and abstractions: Local fMRI activity indexes semantics, not lexical categories. Brain and Language, 132, 28–42. 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.001
(2019). Mplus. The comprehensive modelling program for applied researchers: User’s guide (Vol. 5). Muthén & Muthén.
(2021). Comprehensive stereotype content dictionaries using a semi-automated method. European Journal of Social Psychology, 51(1), 178–196. 10.1002/ejsp.2724
(1957). The measure-ment of meaning. University of Illinois Press.
(2009). Semantic differential technique in the comparative study of cultures. American Anthropologist, 66(3), 171–200. 10.1525/aa.1964.66.3.02a00880
(2019). The big two dictionaries: Capturing agency and communion in natural language. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(5), 871–887. 10.1525/aa.1964.66.3.02a00880
(2019). The big two dictionaries:Capturing agency and communion in natural language. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(5), 871–887. 10.1002/ejsp.2561
(2023, April 12). Data for Study 1 and Study 2. “Semantic and syntactic agency.” https://osf.io/r2znv
(2017). Go pro bono: Prosocial language as a success factor in crowdfunding. Social Psychology, 48(5), 265–278. 10.1027/1864-9335/a000319
(2019). Subtle linguistic cues increase girls’ engagement in science. Psychological Science, 30(3), 455–466. 10.1177/0956797618823670
(2017). Connotation frames of power and agency in modern films. Proceedings of the 2017 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (pp. 2329–2334). 10.18653/v1/D17-1247
(2006)Effects of age and gender on blogging. AAAI Spring Symposium: Computational Approaches to Analyzing Weblogs (Vol. 6, pp. 199–205). AAAI.
(2019). The Glasgow Norms: Ratings of 5,500 words on nine scales. Behavior Research Methods, 51(3), 1258–1270. 10.3758/s13428-018-1099-3
(1988). The cognitive functions of linguistic categories in describing persons: Social cognition and language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(4), 558–568. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.558
(2022). The rise of #climateaction in the time of the FridaysForFuture movement: A semantic network analysis. Social Networks. 10.1016/j.socnet.2022.06.003
(2005). Explaining the nature of power: A three-process theory. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(1), 1–22. 10.1002/ejsp.244
(2013). Empathic perspective taking and the situational malleability of the communal self-concept. Self and Identity, 12(3), 238–258. 10.1080/15298868.2012.655896
(2011). Nouns and verbs in the brain: A review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 407–426. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007
(2020). Natural emotion vocabularies as windows on distress and well-being. Nature Communications, 11(1), Article
(1 . 10.1038/s41467-020-18349-02013). Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45(4), 1191–1207. 10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x
(2022). The verb–self link: An implicit association test study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 29(5), 1946–1959. 10.3758/s13423-022-02105-0
(2022). The evolutionary origins of syntax: Event cognition in nonhuman primates. Science Advances, 8(25), Article
(eabn8464 . 10.1126/sciadv.abn8464