Skip to main content
Open AccessMultistudy Report

Development and Validation of a German Interpersonal Sensitivities Circumplex (ISC-G)

A Self-Report Measure of Perceived Aversive Behaviors of Others

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000737

Abstract

Abstract: The Interpersonal Sensitivities Circumplex (ISC) assesses individual differences in sensitivities to aversive interpersonal behaviors. In this research (total N = 1,519), we developed and validated a German adaptation of the ISC (ISC-G) and extended the nomological net of interpersonal sensitivity as a construct. Using the structural summary method, we investigated associations with self- and informant reports of adaptive (interpersonal) personality traits as well as self-reported personality functioning, maladaptive personality traits, childhood trauma, and hypersensitivity. Replicating and extending previous findings with the ISC, the present research sheds light on the interplay between different personality traits and the perception of others’ interpersonal behavior. Results suggested that individuals report experiencing interpersonal behavior opposite to their own self-description in terms of agency and communion as aversive. This oppositional pattern was most pronounced for antagonistic vs. agreeable traits/behaviors. We discuss these results in the context of research on personality pathology.

As a social species, human beings interact with others regularly. But interpersonal contact is not always pleasant. Sometimes, others’ behavior is irritating. Importantly, not every person is disturbed by the same behavior and in the same intensity as others. While some may be irritated by others’ friendliness, others may welcome it. The Interpersonal Sensitivities Circumplex (ISC; Hopwood et al., 2011) was developed to capture individual differences in sensitivity to different interpersonal behaviors.

In the ISC model, various forms of interpersonal sensitivities are organized within the Interpersonal Circumplex framework (IPC; see Figure 1; Leary, 1957; Wiggins, 1991). The IPC organizes interpersonal phenomena (behaviors, traits, motives) in a circular manner around the two orthogonal dimensions of agency (dominance, power, status) and communion (friendliness, warmth, love). Agency ranges from assertive/dominant to unassertive/submissive behavior and thus reflects the individual’s relative status (“getting ahead”). Communion ranges from warm/agreeable to cold/quarrelsome behavior and thus reflects the extent to which the person “gets along” with others (Wiggins, 1991). Within the IPC, behaviors that are alike are placed side by side, whereas behaviors that are thought to be unrelated are placed at an angle of 90°, and opposite behaviors are placed on opposite sides of the circle. As illustrated in Figure 1, the IPC can be divided into eight segments (octants) labeled with two capital letters (Wiggins, 1979). Each octant represents a specific blend of agency and communion (e.g., the octant NO reflects high agency and high communion). The original English-language ISC is comprised of 64 items representing various kinds of aversive interpersonal behaviors. Respondents are asked to rate how much it bothers them when another person engages in the described behavior (e.g., “doesn’t want to be friends”, “is bossy”). Items are aggregated into eight scales that reflect the octants of the IPC (Figure 1). Each octant represents the tendency to be bothered when others show behaviors representative of that specific blend of agency and communion. In addition, general interpersonal sensitivity (i.e., regardless of style) can be distinguished from these dimensions by considering a total score or general factor.

Figure 1 Circumplex models of Interpersonal Sensitivities Circumplex (ISC; Hopwood et al., 2011; left panel) and Interpersonal Adjective Scales (IAS-R; Wiggins et al., 1988; right panel).

In interpersonal theory, the principle of complementarity describes the tendency of two interacting individuals to behave oppositely in terms of agency and similarly in terms of communion (Kiesler, 1983; Carson, 1969). When the dynamics of interactions are measured as they unfold, research has reliably found that dominant behavior pulls for submissive behavior and submissive behavior pulls for dominant behavior, whereas warmth pulls for warmth and coldness pulls for coldness (Fox et al., 2021; Sadler et al., 2009). It has been theorized that this kind of complementarity occurs because complementary behaviors satisfy the underlying motives of interaction partners (Horowitz et al., 2006). It follows that anticomplementary behaviors (i.e., similar on agency and opposite on communion) should be experienced as bothersome because interpersonal motives are frustrated.

Using the ISC, however, Hopwood et al. (2011) found that – whereas cold-dominant and dominant behaviors were reported as most bothersome on average – individuals tended to find interpersonally opposite behaviors aversive (i.e., opposite on both agency and communion). Thus, participants report being generally less bothered by behaviors they exhibit themselves and bothered more by behaviors contrary to their own. For example, individuals with cold-dominant (BC) interpersonal traits reported relatively greater sensitivities to warm-submissive behaviors (JK, sensitivity to dependence) in the ISC. The Big Five domains of extraversion and agreeableness can be seen as rotated variants of the interpersonal dimensions of agency and communion (McCrae & Costa, 1989), with (high) extraversion being located in the warm-dominant octant (NO) and (high) agreeableness being indicative of warm-submissive behavior (JK). Consistent with the notion of interpersonal opposites, Hopwood et al. (2011) found extraversion to be specifically related to sensitivity to timidity (FG) and agreeableness to be specifically related to sensitivity to antagonism (BC). Thus, the principle of complementarity may not be reflected in the relationship between individuals’ self-reported traits and their descriptions of how they perceive others’ behavior in general. The finding of interpersonal opposites tending to be reported as bothersome in the ISC rather appears to align with social psychological findings regarding the effect of similarity on liking. Specifically, meta-analytic findings suggest that perceived and actual similarities between persons are associated with higher attraction, and thus liking, toward others (Montoya et al., 2008). As the robust effect that ‘similarity attracts’ (Byrne, 1997) also pertains to personality traits (e.g., Grosz et al., 2015; Tenney et al., 2009), its counterpart assumption that dissimilarity generates repulsion (Rosenbaum, 1986) may help explain interpersonal opposites being perceived as disturbing. In this vein, the similarity-attraction literature may provide an alternative hypothesis to the complementarity principle with regard to the perception of interpersonal behaviors: Individuals could indeed find behaviors most aversive that are opposite to their own on both agency and communion.

Personality disorders (PDs) are often described in terms of interpersonal dysfunction (e.g., Hopwood, 2018; Hopwood et al., 2013; Pincus et al., 2020; Wright et al., 2012). For example, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edition; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), borderline PD is characterized by unstable interpersonal relationships, schizoid PD is associated with detachment from social relationships, and individuals with dependent PD fear the loss of support or approval and show submissive behavior in order to elicit warm, caregiving reactions in others (APA, 2013). The Alternative Model for Personality Disorders in DSM-5 (AMPD; APA, 2013) posits two components of personality pathology that capture interpersonal content. Criterion A pertains to general personality functioning and involves interpersonal dysfunction in the form of impairments in an individual’s capacity to relate to others (i.e., empathy, intimacy). Criterion B contains the maladaptive traits and facets model of which several are inherently interpersonal per definition (e.g., hostility, withdrawal) and are also characterized by specific interpersonal content in terms of problematic interpersonal behaviors associated with them (Williams & Simms, 2016; Wright et al., 2012). Research using the ISC has thus far accumulated evidence suggesting that interpersonal sensitivities show general and specific associations with features of personality pathology. For instance, results indicate that more severe personality dysfunction is associated with more general interpersonal sensitivity (Dowgwillo et al., 2018; Hopwood et al., 2018). Other correlates of interpersonal sensitivities include pathological narcissism (Hopwood et al., 2011), obsessive-compulsive PD (Cain et al., 2015), and subclinical maladaptive traits (Dowgwillo & Pincus, 2017; Grove et al., 2019). Interpersonal dysfunction is often studied in terms of interpersonal problems (e.g., Alden et al., 1990), which reflect an individual’s perception of problems with their own interpersonal behavior (e.g., being aggressive toward others; trying to please others too much). Importantly, interpersonal problems and sensitivities appear to be related but distinct sources of information about an individual’s interpersonal dysfunction (Hopwood & Good, 2019).

This research aimed to adapt the ISC (Hopwood et al., 2011) into German, validate the new measure, and further investigate how interpersonal sensitivities relate to personality, interpersonal style, and psychopathology. We followed Hopwood et al. in examining associations between ISC octants and self-reported Big Five domains and interpersonal traits. Extending previous research, we also examined associations between self-reported interpersonal sensitivities and interpersonal traits as judged by groups of informants for each target individual. This enabled more robust interpretations of the interplay between interpersonal style and sensitivities. We also extended previous research by examining maladaptive personality traits in greater detail. Although the link between personality functioning and interpersonal sensitivity has already been established in previous studies, findings are limited to Criterion A of the AMPD and specific maladaptive personality traits such as pathological narcissism. In addition to replicating these correlates, we explored the ISC in relation to Criterion B. Finally, we investigated associations with further variables related to maladaptive social functioning. Childhood maltreatment is characterized by attachment and interpersonal problems in adults (Godbout et al., 2019; Paradis & Boucher, 2010). Hypersensitive individuals report being easily stressed and overwhelmed by internal and external demands and stimuli (Aron & Aron, 1997; Benham, 2006).

We had the following hypotheses. First, we expected that general interpersonal sensitivity would be related to personality dysfunction, pathological narcissism, childhood maltreatment, and hypersensitivity. Second, we expected people to report being most bothered by their interpersonal opposites across interpersonal traits and both normal-range and maladaptive personality traits.

Method

Samples

We tested our hypotheses in four independent samples. Table 1 describes sample characteristics and measures used in each sample. All participants provided informed consent and received course credit (student samples), monetary rewards, or comparable rewards (community and general population samples). Participants were recruited at several universities, online, or via the panel provider Respondi (Sample 4). In Sample 2, informants were recruited by their respective targets. We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures used in this research. In terms of sample size, we included samples that allow for stable estimation of correlations (Schönbrodt & Perugini, 2013). This criterion was not considered for Sample 2, given the complexity of collecting multi-rater data. In Samples 1 and 3, we excluded participants with more than 10% missing values in the ISC. In Samples 2 and 4, there were no missing data. Sample 1 was used to derive a 40-item ISC from a larger item pool. All other samples were used for validation analyses.

Table 1 Sample characteristics and overview of measures used in specific samples

Measures

In the following, all measures will be listed in the order of the respective samples in which they were used. In Sample 1, only the German ISC item pool was administered. In further samples, the final ISC-G was assessed alongside the criterion measures.

Interpersonal Sensitivities Circumplex – German Item Pool (Sample 1) and ISC-G (Further Samples)

We aimed to construct a German adaptation of the ISC (Hopwood et al., 2011), a 64-item questionnaire with items assessing the extent to which individuals are bothered by others’ interpersonal behaviors. In the ISC, respondents are asked to rate how much it bothers them when another individual engages in the behaviors described by the items, using a response format ranging from 1 = not at all, never to 8 = extremely, always. Items form eight scales (i.e., octants), each representing different types of interpersonal sensitivity (e.g., sensitivity to affection, LM), and are organized within the circular structure of the IPC.

For a pilot study (N = 206 students, 80% female, MAge = 24.7), the 64 original English items were translated into German, such that wordings remained as similar as possible. The German version was then back-translated by native English speakers to check for correspondence, and the final translation was approved by the first author of the original form. However, the 64 original items did not perform well, which was primarily due to the low reliability of affiliative octant scales (i.e., NO, LM, JK) and poor model fit. We thus reworded the 25 most ill-fitting items and also created 33 new items to be able to draw from a larger item pool. A major strategy was to include more extreme descriptions of behaviors in affiliative octant scales, for example, by changing “tells me they love me” to “tells me they love me all the time” (Item 23, LM). In doing so, we attempted to represent extreme behaviors by high intensity (“very”) or frequency (“always”) rather than by statements regarding maladaptivity (“too much”). We aimed for a shorter adaptation that still covers the breadth of ISC octants but also facilitates economic assessment. The revised 97-item set was presented to Sample 1, whereas the final 40-item ISC-G was presented to all further samples.

Interpersonal Adjective List (IAL) Self- and Informant Reports

The IAL (Jacobs & Scholl, 2005) is a German adaptation of the Interpersonal Adjective Scales (IAS; Wiggins, 1995; Wiggins et al., 1988), which measures interpersonal traits using an IPC framework. It has 64 items, eight for each octant scale. Respondents are asked to rate how much a given interpersonal adjective (e.g., assertive, PA) describes them on an 8-point response scale ranging from 1 = extremely inaccurate to 8 = extremely accurate. In the present study, the IAL was used as a self-report (Sample 2a) as well as an informant-report measure (Sample 2b). Cronbach’s α for self-reported octant scales (Mdn = .81) ranged from .75 (JK) to .85 (FG). Informant ratings were averaged on the item level for each target across informants. For averaged informant ratings, α coefficients (Mdn = .83) ranged from .78 (NO) to .86 (PA). Self-informant correlations (Mdn = .51) for octant scales ranged from .31 (JK) to .60 (BC), indicating typical levels of agreement of personality traits between self-reports and informant ratings (e.g., Connelly & Ones, 2010).

Minimum Redundancy Scales (MRS)

The 30-item version of the MRS (Ostendorf, 1990; Schallberger & Venetz, 1999) assesses the Big Five personality domains via 30 pairs of adjectives optimized to have as little semantic overlap as possible. For each item, respondents are asked to rate which of the two adjectives describes them better on a 6-point bipolar scale ranging from a strong endorsement of the first term (1) to a strong endorsement of the second term (6). One adjective reflects high trait levels of the respective domain, and the other reflects low levels, respectively. In Sample 3, Cronbach’s α coefficients (Mdn = .81) ranged from .78 (agreeableness) to .90 (neuroticism).

Personality Inventory for DSM-5 (PID-5)

The PID-5 is a 220-item self-report measure assessing the dimensional trait model of DSM-5 (APA, 2013; Zimmermann et al., 2014), that is, Criterion B of the AMPD. It assesses 25 maladaptive personality trait facets, with each facet comprising 4 to 14 items. Five groups of three facets each can be combined into the five broader maladaptive trait domains of negative affectivity, detachment, antagonism, disinhibition, and psychoticism. Items are rated on a 4-point response scale ranging from 0 = very false to 3 = very true. In Sample 3, Cronbach’s α of the trait domains (Mdn = .94) ranged from .88 (antagonism) to .95 (psychoticism). For the trait facets, α coefficients (Mdn = .85) ranged from .71 (irresponsibility; manipulativeness) to .95 (eccentricity).

Inventory of Personality Organization (IPO-16)

The IPO-16 (Zimmermann et al., 2013) is a 16-item self-report measure assessing the general severity of an individual’s personality dysfunction (Zimmermann et al., 2020) and therefore served as an indicator of Criterion A of the AMPD. Items describe impairments in three domains of personality functioning (identity diffusion, primitive defenses, and reality testing) and are rated on a 5-point response scale ranging from 1 = never applies to 5 = always applies. In Sample 3, Cronbach’s α of the total score was .84.

Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ)

The CTQ (Bernstein et al., 2003; Wingenfeld et al., 2010) is a 28-item self-report measure assessing different types of abuse (emotional abuse, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional neglect, physical neglect) that may have been experienced during childhood or adolescence. Respondents indicate the frequency of abuse on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = never to 5 = very often. Cronbach’s α of the CTQ total score was .93 in Sample 3.

Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale (HSNS)

The HSNS (Hendin & Cheek, 1997) is a 10-item self-report measure that assesses hypersensitive narcissism as the disposition toward hypersensitivity to criticism, narcissistic vulnerability, and entitlement. We used a German translation of the HSNS provided by the original authors. Items are answered on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 = very uncharacteristic or untrue, strongly disagree to 5 = very characteristic or true, strongly agree. In Sample 4, Cronbach’s α was .78.

Highly Sensitive Person Scale – German (HSPS-G)

The HSPS-G (Konrad & Herzberg, 2019) is a German 26-item adaptation of the HSPS (Aron & Aron, 1997) and assesses hypersensitivity regarding external and internal stimuli via self-report. Items are answered on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 = hardly at all to 4 = extremely. Cronbach’s α of the total score was .94 in Sample 4.

Statistical Analysis

We used the statistical software R 4.1.2 (R Core Team, 2021) for all analyses. More detailed explanations of the statistical analyses are provided in the supplement (Note S1; https://osf.io/mf2c3/). The circumplexity of the ISC-G was evaluated in two ways. First, we used RANDALL’s correspondence index (Tracey, 1997) to determine how well correlations between both raw and ipsatized octants (i.e., after accounting for a general factor) conformed to a circular pattern (e.g., Alden et al., 1990; Hopwood et al., 2011). Second, we tested model fit to a perfect circumplex structure (e.g., Gurtman & Pincus, 2003; Wendt et al., 2019) in confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using the robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) by considering established recommendations for model fit indices (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

The structural summary method (SSM; Zimmermann & Wright, 2017) was used to represent the patterns of correlations between ISC octant scales and external criteria as a cosine curve, which can be seen as projecting a criterion variable onto the circular surface of the ISC-G (see supplemental Figure S1; https://osf.io/mf2c3/). Elevation reflects the criterion’s association with general sensitivity to aversive behaviors. Bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) with 2,000 replicates were used to decide whether elevation is significantly different from zero. The cosine curve’s amplitude indicates the interpersonal specificity of a variable. For interpreting effect sizes of amplitudes, Zimmermann and Wright (2017) conducted an empirical review of amplitudes across the literature, according to which amplitudes around .10, .16, and .23 can be conceived as relatively small, average, or large effects. Angular displacement (from 0°; see Figure 1) reflects the location of the predominant interpersonal theme associated with a criterion within the circumplex. We only took this parameter into account for criteria with a pattern of correlations showing prototypicality (R2; Gurtman & Pincus, 2003), meaning that the pattern of correlations forms a cosine wave. Similar to Hopwood et al. (2011), we used an R2 value of .70 as the cutoff for interpreting displacement.

Results

Deriving the Final German ISC (ISC-G)

Similar to existing IPC-based measures, the 40-item ISC was derived by projecting the initial item pool on the IPC in Sample 1 and extracting two orthogonal factors (i.e., agency and communion) from ipsatized items using principal components analysis (e.g., Hopwood et al., 2011) and selecting five items per octant. Items were selected to reflect a specific octant well with respect to angular displacement, amplitude, and fit. In the item selection process, we prioritized a combination of high amplitudes (i.e., high communalities in the two-factorial principal components analysis) on the one hand and an average deviation around 0° from an octant’s theoretical angular displacement across items of an octant scale on the other hand. The final ISC-G includes 11 revised, 7 new, and 22 directly translated ISC items (see supplemental Table S1 for a full item list of the ISC-G; https://osf.io/mf2c3/).

Structure and Descriptive Statistics of the ISC-G

We calculated internal consistencies and fit to a circumplex model in all samples (see Table 2). Overall, the data supported the circular structure of the ISC-G with acceptable fit both in terms of RANDALL’s correspondence index and fit to the perfect circumplex CFA model. Fit to the latter model was comparable to model fit reported elsewhere (e.g., Wendt et al., 2019) of different versions of the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems (IIP; Alden et al., 1990). Internal consistencies were in the acceptable range as well, with medians across samples ranging from .74 (JK, NO) to .89 (DE).

Table 2 Fit statistics, internal consistencies, and scale statistics of the ISC-G in all samples

In line with findings from Hopwood et al. (2011), an inspection of scale means showed that participants consistently were most sensitive to control (PA; Mdn = 6.38), antagonism (BC; Mdn = 6.18), and attention-seeking (NO; Mdn = 6.12) but reported relatively little sensitivity to timidity (FG; Mdn = 3.51). In terms of scale usage, individuals thus reported being “quite, frequently” bothered (= 6 on the response scale) by dominant (PA), cold-dominant (BC), and warm-dominant (NO) behaviors, whereas they were “somewhat, more than half of the time” (= 5) bothered by cold behaviors, and “slightly, less than half of the time” (= 4) bothered by cold-submissive (FG), warm (LM), warm-submissive (JK), and submissive (HI) behaviors. With respect to the higher-order dimensions of agency and communion, only 48 (3%) of all 1,519 participants across samples reported being relatively more bothered by submissive behaviors (i.e., negative values on the higher-order dimension of agency), whereas 1,471 (97%) were relatively more bothered by dominant behaviors (i.e., positive values on agency). In contrast, participants were almost equally distributed in terms of whether they found warm (779 individuals, 51%) or cold behaviors (740 individuals, 49%) more disturbing.

Associations With Criterion Variables

We used the structural summary method (SSM) to project the criterion variables on the circular ISC surface. We report all correlation-based SSM statistics and their 95% confidence intervals in the supplement (Table S2; https://osf.io/mf2c3/). Prototypical correlation profiles (i.e., R2 ≥ .70) are displayed in Figures 2 and 3.

Figure 2 Projecting Self- and Informant Reported Interpersonal Traits on the ISC-G in Sample 2. Self-ratings on the IAL are displayed on the left and (averaged) informant ratings are displayed on the right. IAL ratings were projected on the ISC-G using the structural summary method with bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals. IAL gregarious-extraverted (NO) is not displayed due to low prototypicality in both ratings (R2 < .70). For ISC octant labels, please see Figure 1, left panel.
Figure 3 Projecting Normal-Range and Maladaptive Personality Traits, Personality Dysfunction, and Adverse Childhood Experiences on the ISC-G in Sample 3. Only patterns of associations characterized by prototypicality (R2 ≥ .70) are displayed. For ISC octant labels, please see Figure 1, left panel.

Self- and Informant Reported Interpersonal Traits

The overall patterns of self- and informant ratings were similar (Figure 2). In general, all self- and informant-rated IAL octants except attention-seeking (NO) showed prototypical profiles. Interpersonal traits ranging counterclockwise from submissive to warm traits (i.e., HI to LM) were associated with relatively greater sensitivity to cold-dominant behaviors. Interpersonal traits ranging counterclockwise from dominant to cold-submissive traits (i.e., PA to FG) were associated with greater sensitivity to warm-submissive behaviors. Although no self-rated interpersonal trait exhibited significant elevation, targets who were judged as assured-dominant (PA; e = .14 [.04, .24]) or arrogant-calculating (BC; e = .14 [.02, .25]) by their informants reported higher overall sensitivities as indicated by higher scores across ISC octants. By contrast, targets who were judged as unassured-submissive by informants reported overall lower sensitivities (HI; e = −.11 [−.21, −.01]).

Normal-Range Personality Traits

MRS neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness exhibited prototypical profiles (Figure 3), whereas conscientiousness and openness did not. Neuroticism (a = .16 [.11, .21], δ = 136° [117°, 156°]) and agreeableness (a = .10 [.05, .16], δ = 119° [88°, 155°]) were associated with sensitivity to antagonism (BC), and extraversion (a = 14. [.08, .19], δ = 216° [194°, 239°]) was associated with sensitivity to timidity (FG). In addition, neuroticism showed a significant elevation (e = .09 [.03, .15]).

Maladaptive Personality Traits and Facets

Of the PID-5 domains with sufficient prototypicality (Figure 3), negative affectivity (a = .20 [.15, .25], δ = 159° [143°, 174°]) was associated with sensitivity to antagonism (BC), and antagonism (a = .10 [.05, .15], δ = 263° [232°, 299°]) was associated with sensitivity to passivity (HI). Negative affectivity (e = .11 [.05, .16]) and detachment (e = .08 [.01, .14]) were further associated with general sensitivity.

PID-5 facets that can be grouped under the same domain according to meta-analytical associations (Somma et al., 2019) generally exhibited the same interpersonal themes but differed with respect to prototypicality and specificity (Figure 3). Separation insecurity (a = .23 [.18, .28], δ = 174° [162°, 186°]) stood out as the facet with the strongest specificity with sensitivity to remoteness (DE). All facets of negative affectivity tended to peak at sensitivities to cold and cold-dominant behaviors. Although detachment and disinhibition did not show prototypicality at the domain level, some of their facets were characterized by predominant interpersonal themes. Among detachment facets, withdrawal (a = .16 [.11, .22], δ = 21° [1°, 42°]) and restricted affectivity (a = .10 [.05, .16], δ = 0° [329°, 30°]) peaked at sensitivities to warm and warm-dominant behaviors. Among disinhibition facets, risk-taking (a = .13 [.08, .18], δ = 279° [252°, 306°]) was associated with sensitivity to passivity (HI). A slightly greater differentiation was found for facets of antagonism. Whereas attention-seeking (a = .12 [.07, .17], δ = 242° [215°, 276°]) and manipulativeness (a = .11 [.07, .16], δ = 254° [225°, 287°]) peaked in the range of sensitivities to submissive or cold-submissive behaviors, callousness (a = .11 [.06, .17], δ = 329° [295°, 3°]) was associated with sensitivity to dependence (JK). Many facets were associated with heightened elevation, but hostility (e = .15 [.10, .20]) and suspiciousness (e = .13 [.07, .19]) showed particularly strong elevations while also not being characterized by a predominant interpersonal theme.

Personality Dysfunction, Adverse Childhood Experiences, Hypersensitive Narcissism, and Hypersensitivity to Stimuli

All criteria for which we expected associations with general sensitivity exhibited significant elevations, including the severity of personality dysfunction as measured by the IPO-16 (e = .11 [.06, .17], Sample 3), adverse childhood experiences as reported in the CTQ (e = .11 [.05, .17], Sample 3), hypersensitive narcissism as measured by the HSNS (e = .33 [.26, .39], Sample 4), and HSPS-G generalized hypersensitivity (e = .35 [.29, .41], Sample 4). Personality dysfunction further peaked at sensitivities to cold or cold-dominant behaviors (a = .10 [.06, .16], δ = 165° [131°, 197°]). Interestingly, childhood maltreatment (a = .10 [.05, .17], δ = 32° [2°, 64°]) also exhibited specificity in terms of sensitivity to warm-dominant behavior as interpersonal content.

Discussion

This research aimed to construct and validate a German adaptation of the ISC (Hopwood et al., 2011). Overall, we achieved our goal of a shorter 40-item ISC-G that exhibits an acceptable fit to a perfect circumplex structure. Using various measures of constructs established in clinical and personality science, we found associations with general and specific sensitivities that align with previous research. For example, we replicated the link between general personality dysfunction and general interpersonal sensitivity, indicating that individuals with greater impairments in personality functioning tend to be more sensitive with respect to others’ interpersonal behaviors regardless of the specific interpersonal style (Dowgwillo et al., 2018; Hopwood et al., 2018). We extended the nomological net of interpersonal sensitivities by investigating associations with the maladaptive traits and facets model of DSM-5, experiences of childhood maltreatment, and general hypersensitivity, of which the latter two were also characterized by elevated overall sensitivity.

Interpersonal Sensitivities and Opposites

Perhaps the most important findings pertained to the patterns of specific associations between traits and sensitivities. Interpersonal theory posits the principle of complementarity (e.g., Carson, 1969) for dyadic interactions, according to which behaviors opposite on communion and similar on agency should be perceived as particularly bothersome. Hopwood et al. (2011) first presented results indicating that individuals report being irritated by interpersonal opposites (i.e., behaviors opposite on both agency and communion) when asked to fill out the ISC outside of a specific interactional context. Thus, complementarity may not pertain to self-descriptions of individuals’ traits. In the present study, the strongest evidence of interpersonal opposites being reported as most bothersome was provided by IAL projections onto the ISC, which showed that this pattern applied to self-reported interpersonal styles and ratings by three informants per subject. As self- and informant ratings produced similar results, we filled the gap of Hopwood et al. (2011) questioning the reliance on self-reports of interpersonal style. We also directly replicated the previously reported interpersonal sensitivities associated with Big Five neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness. Maladaptive traits and facets of the PID-5 also conformed to a pattern of opposites when considering previous findings of problematic interpersonal behaviors consistently associated with many of them (Williams & Simms, 2016; Wright et al., 2012). All traits and facets that showed prototypicality for specific interpersonal problems in previous research and specific interpersonal sensitivities in Sample 3 mapped onto the opposite octant on the IPC (see supplemental Figure S2; https://osf.io/mf2c3/). For instance, individuals high in antagonism tend to report problems with being too dominant themselves. In the ISC-G, such individuals reported being bothered by submissive behavior. The PID-5 facet of restricted affectivity is associated with problems of being too cold and exhibited prototypicality for sensitivity to warmth in this study. However, it remains to be seen whether these patterns emerge with such oppositional symmetry when tested in the same sample. For example, similar to previous results by Hopwood et al. (2011), individuals’ own interpersonal traits did not directly map onto the opposite octant in each case in Sample 2, even though all patterns corresponded to a general notion of opposition in that interpersonal traits were associated with sensitivities to behaviors on the opposite side of at least one or both axes. Thus, more research is needed to examine the level of precision with which patterns of interpersonal opposites being most bothersome apply to traits.

Overall, the perception of interpersonal opposites being disturbing in research using the ISC seems compatible with the robust similarity-attraction effect (Byrne, 1997). In particular, individuals tending to be bothered by behaviors dissimilar to their own in terms of agency and communion best aligns with the assumption that – akin to similarity generating liking – dissimilarity generates repulsion (Rosenbaum, 1986). However, it should be noted that this latter effect is less empirically established and that it is rather unclear whether dissimilarity actually leads to disliking or whether it merely does not lead to liking (e.g., Sprecher, 2019). Furthermore, in our view, conclusions with respect to the effects of (dis)similarity are limited by the response format of the ISC. In the questionnaire, respondents only indicate the degree to which they are bothered by a respective behavior, which does not necessarily convey a degree of liking for the opposite or the same behavior (e.g., indicating being “not at all” bothered when someone “cannot assert themselves” may merely reflect indifference to said behavior and does not provide information about liking for dominant behavior). The exact relationship between the tendencies to like and dislike the behaviors rated in the ISC remains an open research question.

Taking into account all criteria we investigated, it becomes evident that specific interpersonal sensitivities were mostly located on the diagonal axis of the IPC, ranging from cold-dominant (BC) to warm-submissive (JK) behavior (and thus representing antagonism vs. agreeableness). The other diagonal axis, ranging from cold-submissive (FG) to warm-dominant (NO) behavior (and thus representing introversion vs. extraversion), was less frequently associated with specific sensitivities. Still, criteria such as extraversion and its maladaptive PID-5 facet variant of attention-seeking, for instance, were associated with sensitivity to their supposed opposite of timidity (FG). The only criteria characterized by sensitivities toward warm-dominant behavior were the PID-5 facet of withdrawal and CTQ adverse childhood experiences. Given that individuals with adverse childhood experiences can exhibit problems with being interpersonally distant and nonassertive toward others in their own behavior (Huh et al., 2014; Maier et al., 2020; Paradis & Boucher, 2010), which corresponds to a cold-submissive style, this could also be in line with an oppositional pattern.

Future Directions

In future studies, the limitations of this research should be addressed. First, cross-cultural and cross-language equivalence needs to be considered, given that the translated original items of the ISC-64 yielded rather poor results in the pilot study. The ISC-G, however, replicated and extended several central findings of the English version in an expected manner and thus appears to be a useful tool to study the construct of interpersonal sensitivity. It is noteworthy that similar problems of a direct translation affected other IPC-based measures, such as the German adaption (Thomas et al., 2012) of the Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Values (CSIV; Locke, 2000). This highlights the need to ensure cultural fit for items assessing constructs from the interpersonal realm. In the present case of interpersonal sensitivity, divergences between the English and German versions mainly pertained to the affiliative octant scales. These could be rooted in differing cultural norms concerning appropriate affiliative behavior. In general, German items needed to be more extreme; for example, with respect to the intensity of behaviors (e.g., ISC-64 Item 31: “Expresses concern about me” worked better in German when framed as Item 39: “Always expresses concern about me”). To further examine these differences, the translated ISC-G with its revised and new items as compared to the ISC-64 could be tested in US samples.

The fact that indicators of both Criterion A (IPO-16) and B (PID-5 traits and facets) of the AMPD could be described by general and/or specific sensitivities highlights once more the importance of interpersonal dysfunction within personality pathology (e.g., Benjamin, 2002; Hopwood et al., 2013; Pincus, 2005). Until now, the closest proxy of subjects’ actual interpersonal behavior was the combination of self-reported and informant-rated interpersonal styles in Sample 2. However, a diary study would have the potential to explicitly highlight the effects of interpersonal sensitivity on individuals’ perception of interpersonal interaction. For example, ambulatory assessment methods could be used to examine whether a baseline assessment of interpersonal sensitivity predicts certain behavioral and experiential patterns in subjects’ everyday life (Lewis & Ridenour, 2020; Roche et al., 2014). For instance, highly sensitive individuals could encounter many irritating interpersonal situations or avoid them and just encounter few, which then impact even more heavily.

The ISC-G should also be assessed in clinical samples to test whether the measure distinguishes between healthy individuals and patients. Thus far, results using the ISC suggest that psychotherapy could benefit from insights into patients’ sensitivities provided by the ISC, particularly in the presence of personality pathology. Although it is established that therapists’ ability to relate to patients in a warm and empathic manner is important for a therapeutic alliance (Ackerman & Hilsenroth, 2003; Nienhuis et al., 2018), research using the ISC shows that warm behavior can also be aversive to some patients and therefore threaten the therapeutic relationship.

Conclusion

Herein, we constructed a German adaptation of the ISC and used the new measure to gain a better understanding of the construct. Results suggest that individuals generally find behaviors aversive that are dissimilar to their own personality and that interpersonal sensitivity may be important to understanding interpersonal dysfunction in individuals with personality pathology. This insight can be used for future research investigating interpersonal dysfunction, its antecedents, and consequences.

Parts of this research were presented as a poster at the 16th Annual Meeting of the Society for Interpersonal Theory and Research (SITAR). We thank Katrin Keßler and Daniel Leising for their contributions in the early stages of this project.

References

  • Ackerman, S. J., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2003). A review of therapist characteristics and techniques positively impacting the therapeutic alliance. Clinical Psychology Review, 23(1), 1–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00146-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Alden, L. E., Wiggins, J. S., & Pincus, A. L. (1990). Construction of circumplex scales for the Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55(3–4), 521–536. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.1990.9674088 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.). APA. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.893619 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1997). Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion and emotionality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(2), 345–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.345 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benham, G. (2006). The highly sensitive person: Stress and physical symptom reports. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(7), 1433–1440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.11.021 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benjamin, L. S. (2002). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of personality disorders (2nd ed.). Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., Stokes, J., Handelsman, L., Medrano, M., Desmond, D., & Zule, W. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 169–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00541-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Byrne, D. (1997). An overview (and underview) of research and theory within the attraction paradigm. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14(3), 417–431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407597143008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cain, N. M., Ansell, E. B., Simpson, H. B., & Pinto, A. (2015). Interpersonal functioning in obsessive–compulsive personality disorder. Journal of Personality Assessment, 97(1), 90–99. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2014.934376 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carson, R. C. (1969). Interaction concepts of personality. Aldine. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Connelly, B. S., & Ones, D. S. (2010). An other perspective on personality: Meta-analytic integration of observers’ accuracy and predictive validity. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 1092–1122. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021212 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dowgwillo, E. A., & Pincus, A. L. (2017). Differentiating dark triad traits within and across interpersonal circumplex surfaces. Assessment, 24(1), 24–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191116643161 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dowgwillo, E. A., Roche, M. J., & Pincus, A. L. (2018). Examining the interpersonal nature of criterion A of the DSM-5 section III alternative model for personality disorders using bootstrapped confidence intervals for the interpersonal circumplex. Journal of Personality Assessment, 100(6), 581–592. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2018.1464016 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fox, S. E., Thomas, K. M., Durbin, C. E., & Hopwood, C. J. (2021). A variance decomposition of the continuous assessment of interpersonal dynamics. Psychological Assessment, 33(5), 427–442. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000995 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Godbout, N., Daspe, M.-È., Runtz, M., Cyr, G., & Briere, J. (2019). Childhood maltreatment, attachment, and borderline personality-related symptoms: Gender-specific structural equation models. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 11(1), 90–98. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000403 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grosz, M. P., Dufner, M., Back, M. D., & Denissen, J. J. (2015). Who is open to a narcissistic romantic partner? The roles of sensation seeking, trait anxiety, and similarity. Journal of Research in Personality, 58, 84–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2015.05.007 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Grove, J. L., Smith, T. W., Girard, J. M., & Wright, A. G. (2019). Narcissistic admiration and rivalry: An interpersonal approach to construct validation. Journal of Personality Disorders, 33(6), 751–775. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2019_33_374 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gurtman, M. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2003). The circumplex model: Methods and research applications. In J. SchinkaW. VelicerEds., Handbook of psychology, Vol. 2: Research methods in psychology (pp. 407–428). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/0471264385.wei0216 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hendin, H. M., & Cheek, J. M. (1997). Assessing hypersensitive narcissism: A reexamination of Murray’s Narcism Scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 31(4), 588–599. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1997.2204 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopwood, C. J. (2018). A framework for treating DSM‐5 alternative model for personality disorder features. Personality and Mental Health, 12(2), 107–125. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1414 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopwood, C. J., Ansell, E. B., Pincus, A. L., Wright, A. G., Lukowitsky, M. R., & Roche, M. J. (2011). The circumplex structure of interpersonal sensitivities. Journal of Personality, 79(4), 707–740. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2011.00696.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopwood, C. J., Good, E. W., & Morey, L. C. (2018). Validity of the DSM-5 levels of Personality Functioning Scale – self report. Journal of Personality Assessment, 100(6), 650–659. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2017.1420660 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopwood, C. J., & Good, E. W. (2019). Structure and correlates of interpersonal problems and sensitivities. Journal of Personality, 87(4), 843–855. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12437 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopwood, C. J., Wright, A. G., Ansell, E. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2013). The interpersonal core of personality pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27(3), 270–295. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2013.27.3.270 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Horowitz, L. M., Wilson, K. R., Turan, B., Zolotsev, P., Constantino, M. J., & Henderson, L. (2006). How interpersonal motives clarify the meaning of interpersonal behavior: A revised circumplex model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(1), 67–86. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Huh, H. J., Kim, S. Y., Yu, J. J., & Chae, J. H. (2014). Childhood trauma and adult interpersonal relationship problems in patients with depression and anxiety disorders. Annals of General Psychiatry, 13, Article 26. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12991-014-0026-y First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobs, I., & Scholl, W. (2005). Interpersonale Adjektivliste (IAL): Die empirische Umsetzung theoretischer Circumplex-Eigenschaften für die Messung interpersonaler Stile [Interpersonal Adjective List (IAL): An empirical realization of theoretical circumplex concepts for the assessment of interpersonal style]. Diagnostica, 51(3), 145–155. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924.51.3.145 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Kiesler, D. J. (1983). The 1982 Interpersonal Circle: A taxonomy for complementarity in human transactions. Psychological Review, 90(3), 185–214. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.3.185 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Konrad, S., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2019). Psychometric properties and validation of a German High Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS-G). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 35(3), 364–378. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000411 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Leary, T. (1957). Interpersonal diagnosis of personality: A functional theory and methodology for personality evaluation. Ronald Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Leising, D., Borkenau, P., Zimmermann, J., Roski, C., Leonhardt, A., & Schutz, A. (2013). Positive self‐regard and claim to leadership: Two fundamental forms of self‐evaluation. European Journal of Personality, 27(6), 565–579. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1924 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lewis, K. C., & Ridenour, J. M. (2020). The integration of EMA and single-occasion multimethod assessment data for a complex psychiatric patient. Assessment, 27(7), 1532–1546. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191118825313 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Locke, K. D. (2000). Circumplex scales of interpersonal values: Reliability, validity, and applicability to interpersonal problems and personality disorders. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75(2), 249–267. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7502_6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maier, A., Gieling, C., Heinen-Ludwig, L., Stefan, V., Schultz, J., Güntürkün, O., Becker, B., Hurlemann, R., & Scheele, D. (2020). Association of childhood maltreatment with interpersonal distance and social touch preferences in adulthood. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(1), 37–46. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19020212 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1989). The structure of interpersonal traits: Wiggins’s circumplex and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(4), 586–595. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.4.586 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., & Kirchner, J. (2008). Is actual similarity necessary for attraction? A meta-analysis of actual and perceived similarity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 25(6), 889–922. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407508096700 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, S. (2022). Development and validation of a German Interpersonal Sensitivities Circumplex (ISC-G): A self-report measure of perceived aversive behaviors of others. https://osf.io/mf2c3 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nienhuis, J. B., Owen, J., Valentine, J. C., Winkeljohn Black, S., Halford, T. C., Parazak, S. E., Budge, S., & Hilsenroth, M. (2018). Therapeutic alliance, empathy, and genuineness in individual adult psychotherapy: A meta-analytic review. Psychotherapy Research, 28(4), 593–605. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2016.1204023 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ostendorf, F. (1990). Sprache und Persönlichkeitsstruktur. Zur Validität des Fünf-Faktoren-Modells der Persönlichkeit [Language and personality structure: Towards the validity of the Five-Factor model of personality]. S. Roderer. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Paradis, A., & Boucher, S. (2010). Child maltreatment history and interpersonal problems in adult couple relationships. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 19(2), 138–158. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926770903539433 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pincus, A. L. (2005). A contemporary integrative interpersonal theory of personality disorders. In M. F. LenzenwegerJ. F. ClarkinEds., Major theories of personality disorder (pp. 282–331). Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pincus, A. L., Hopwood, C. J., & Wright, A. G. C. (2020). The interpersonal situation: An integrative framework for the study of personality, psychopathology, and psychotherapy. In D. FunderJ. F. RauthmannR. ShermanEds., Oxford handbook of psychological situations (pp. 124–142). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Roche, M. J., Pincus, A. L., Rebar, A. L., Conroy, D. E., & Ram, N. (2014). Enriching psychological assessment using a person-specific analysis of interpersonal processes in daily life. Assessment, 21(5), 515–528. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191114540320 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenbaum, M. E. (1986). The repulsion hypothesis: On the nondevelopment of relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1156–1166. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1156 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sadler, P., Ethier, N., Gunn, G. R., Duong, D., & Woody, E. (2009). Are we on the same wavelength? Interpersonal complementarity as shared cyclical patterns during interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 1005–1020. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016232 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schallberger, U., & Venetz, M. (1999). Kurzversionen des MRS-Inventars von Ostendorf (1990) zur Erfassung der fünf „großen“ Persönlichkeitsfakoren [Brief versions of Ostendorf’s (1990) MRS inventory for assessing the five “big” personality factors]. University of Zürich. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Somma, A., Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., & Fossati, A. (2019). The replicability of the Personality Inventory for DSM-5 domain scale factor structure in US and non-U.S. samples: A quantitative review of the published literature. Psychological Assessment, 31(7), 861–877. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000711 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sprecher, S. (2019). Does (dis)similarity information about a new acquaintance lead to liking or repulsion? An experimental test of a classic social psychology issue. Social Psychology Quarterly, 82(3), 303–318. https://doi.org/10.1177/0190272519855954 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tenney, E. R., Turkheimer, E., & Oltmanns, T. F. (2009). Being liked is more than having a good personality: The role of matching. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(4), 579–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thomas, A., Locke, K. D., & Strauß, B. (2012). Das Inventar zur Erfassung interpersonaler Motive (IIM) [Scales of interpersonal values IIM: Development and validation of a German version of the Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Values]. Diagnostica, 58(4), 211–226. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000074 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Tracey, T. J. G. (1997). RANDALL: A Microsoft FORTRAN program for a randomization test of hypothesized order relations. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57(1), 164–168. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164497057001012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wendt, L. P., Wright, A., Pilkonis, P. A., Nolte, T., Fonagy, P., Montague, P. R., Benecke, C., Krieger, T., & Zimmermann, J. (2019). The latent structure of interpersonal problems: Validity of dimensional, categorical, and hybrid models. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128(8), 823–839. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000460 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiggins, J. S. (1979). A psychological taxonomy of trait-descriptive terms: The interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(3), 395–412. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.3.395 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wiggins, J. S. (1991). Agency and communion as conceptual coordinates for the understanding and measurement of interpersonal behavior. In W. GroveD. CicchettiEds., Thinking clearly about psychology: Essays in honor of Paul E. Meehl (Vol. 2, pp. 89–113). University of Minnesota Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wiggins, J. S. (1995). Interpersonal Adjective Scales: Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wiggins, J. S., Trapnell, P., & Phillips, N. (1988). Psychometric and geometric characteristics of the Revised Interpersonal Adjective Scales (IAS-R). Multivariate Behavioral Research, 23(4), 517–530. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2304_8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, T. F., & Simms, L. J. (2016). Personality disorder models and their coverage of interpersonal problems. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 7(1), 15–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000140 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wingenfeld, K., Spitzer, C., Mensebach, C., Grabe, H. J., Hill, A., Gast, U., Schlosser, N., Höpp, H., Beblo, T., & Driessen, M. (2010). Die deutsche Version des Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ): Erste Befunde zu den psychometrischen Kennwerten [The German version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ): Preliminary psychometric properties]. Psychotherapie – Psychosomatik – Medizinische Psychologie, 60(11), 442–450. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0030-1247564 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wright, A. G. C., Pincus, A. L., Hopwood, C. J., Thomas, K. M., Markon, K. E., & Krueger, R. F. (2012). An interpersonal analysis of pathological personality traits in DSM-5. Assessment, 19(3), 263–275. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191112446657 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, J., Altenstein, D., Krieger, T., Holtforth, M. G., Pretsch, J., Alexopoulos, J., Spitzer, C., Benecke, C., Krueger, R. F., Markon, K. E., & Leising, D. (2014). The structure and correlates of self-reported DSM-5 maladaptive personality traits: Findings from two German-speaking samples. Journal of Personality Disorders, 28(4), 518–540. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_130 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, J., Benecke, C., Hörz, S., Rentrop, M., Peham, D., Bock, A., Wallner, T., Schauenburg, H., Frommer, J., Huber, D., Clarkin, J. F., & Dammann, G. (2013). Validierung einer deutschsprachigen 16-Item-Version des Inventars der Persönlichkeitsorganisation (IPO-16) [Validity of a German 16-item version of the Inventory of Personality Organization (IPO-16)]. Diagnostica, 59(1), 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000076 First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, J., Müller, S., Bach, B., Hutsebaut, J., Hummelen, B., & Fischer, F. (2020). A common metric for self-reported severity of personality disorder. Psychopathology, 53, 161–171. https://doi.org/10.1159/000507377 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, J., & Wright, A. G. (2017). Beyond description in interpersonal construct validation: Methodological advances in the circumplex structural summary approach. Assessment, 24(1), 3–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115621795 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar