Skip to main content
Studie

Einfluss der Aufgabenstellung und des Wortschatzes auf die Emotionserkennung bei Drei- bis Fünfjährigen

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/0942-5403/a000271

Zusammenfassung. Diese Studie untersucht Altersunterschiede in den Fähigkeiten zur Emotionserkennung bei Kindergartenkindern und prüft, inwiefern sich die Aufgabenstellung und der Wortschatz auf diese Aspekte des Emotionswissens auswirken. Hierfür wurde die Fähigkeit zum Benennen und Erkennen von Emotionen anhand von Fotos und vorgegebenen Situationen bei N=170 Drei- bis Fünfjährigen (n=86 Mädchen) mit dem EMK 3 – 6 und der Wortschatz mit dem SET 3 – 5 erfasst. Es wurde ein signifikanter Effekt des Alters und der Aufgabenstellung festgestellt. Der stärkste Zuwachs in der Emotionserkennung zeigte sich in allen Aufgaben im Alter zwischen drei und vier Jahren. Die Erkennungsleistung fiel für Fotos am genauesten aus, am schwersten fiel es Kindern, nach vorgegebenen Situationen den passenden Gesichtsausdruck abzuleiten. Der Wortschatz beeinflusste die Leistungen im Benennen, jedoch nicht im Erkennen von Basisemotionen. Die Ergebnisse lassen sich in die aktuelle Forschung integrieren und liefern wichtige Hinweise für die Diagnostik und Förderung des Emotionswissens.


Effects of Task Assignment and Vocabulary on Emotion Recognition in 3- to 5-Year-Olds

Abstract. Emotion knowledge represents the basis of interpersonal communication and social skills. The ability to recognize or label emotional expression and the ability to infer emotions from situations are central components of emotion knowledge. The aim of this study was to examine developmental changes in these components of emotion knowledge during preschool. The second aim was to investigate to what extent task assignment and vocabulary influence preschoolers’ performance. For this purpose, the data of 170 preschoolers (girls n = 86) were collected using four tasks of the EMK 3 – 6. Furthermore, vocabulary was tested using the SET 3 – 5. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measurement (mode of tasks; label and recognition emotional expressions vs. label and recognition of emotional situations × age groups, 3 – 5 years) was performed. To determine whether performance in four tasks differed between children with and without vocabulary deficits, all scores were entered in a multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA). The ANOVA with repeated measurement confirmed significant age differences across all examined features – labeling emotional expressions: F(2,167) = 12.76, p = .000, η2 = .133; recognizing emotional expression: F(2,167) = 23.02, p = .000, η2 = .216; labeling emotional situations: F(2,167) = 7.82, p = .001, η2 = .086; recognizing emotional situations: F(2,167) = 17.19, p = .000, η2 = .171. Contrast analyses showed significant differences in all emotion competences between 3-year- and 4-year-olds. The 4- and 5-year-olds, however, differed only in emotion recognition tasks. Second, there was also a significant effect of method (F(3,165) = 61.39, p = .000, η2=.527) but no interaction with age. Children performed tasks in this order: recognizing emotional expressions, labeling emotional expressions, labeling emotional situations, and recognizing emotional situations. Results of MANOVA showed that children with vocabulary deficits had significantly lower scores in emotion labeling tasks – labeling emotional expressions: F(1,168) = 6.74, p = .010, η2 = .039; labeling emotional situations: F(1,168) = 6.93, p = .009, η2 = .040 – and no significant differences in emotion recognition tasks. The results highlight relevant improvements in the ability to recognize and label emotions and emotional situations in preschool age children. A hierarchy of difficulty for the tasks was supported and can be integrated into the current research. Further, tasks of the EMK 3 – 6 are well suited to measure different components of emotion knowledge. Children with vocabulary deficits were on average less proficient in labeling tasks. Therefore, the consideration of receptive and expressive language skills ensures that children understand the task instructions, but also allows for a better interpretation of children’s performance and determination of strengths and deficits in the emotional domain. Finally, results provide further evidence of the significant role early promotion of emotional competence plays in early childhood.

Literatur

  • Bakopoulou, I. & Dockrell, J. E. (2016). The role of social cognition and prosocial behaviour in relation to the social-emotional functioning of primary aged children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 49 – 50, 354 – 370. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bassett, H. H., Denham, S. & Mincic, M. (2012). The structure of preschoolers’ emotion knowledge: Model equivalence and validity using a structural equation modeling approach. Early Education and Development, 23, 259 – 279. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beck, L., Kumschick, I. R., Eid, M. & Klann-Delius, G. (2012). Relationship between language competence and emotional competence in middle childhood. Emotion, 12, 503 – 514. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bühner, M. & Ziegler, M. (2017). Statistik für Psychologen und Sozialwissenschaftler (2., akt. u. erw. Aufl.). München: Pearson. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Camras, L. A. & Allison, K. (1985). Children’s understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9, 84 – 94. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Collin, L., Bindra, J., Raju, M., Gillberg, C. & Minnis, H. (2013). Facial emotion recognition in child psychiatry: A systematic review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 1505 – 1520. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cummings, A. J. & Rennels, J. L. (2014). How mood and task complexity affect children’s recognition of others’ emotions. Social Development, 23, 80 – 99. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Stasio, S., Fiorilli, C. & Chiacchio, C. (2014). Effects of verbal ability and fluid intelligence on children’s emotion understanding. International Journal of Psychology, 49, 409 – 414. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. A. (1998). Emotional development in young children. New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., Zinsser, K. & Wyatt, T. M. (2014). How preschoolers’ social-emotional learning predicts their early school success: Developing theory-promoting, competency-based assessments. Infant and Child Development, 23, 426 – 454. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Denham, S. A. & Couchoud, E. A. (1990). Young preschoolers’ understanding of emotions. Child Study Journal, 20, 171 – 192. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ensor, R., Spencer, D. & Hughes, C. (2011). ‘You feel sad?’ emotion understanding mediates effects of verbal ability and mother-child mutuality on prosocial behaviors: Findings from 2 years to 4 years. Social Development, 20, 93 – 110. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fischer, A. H. & Manstead, A. S. R. (2016). Social functions of emotion and emotion. In M. LewiJ. Haviland-Jones L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed.; pp. 424 – 439). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Grossmann, T. (2010). The development of emotion perception in face and voice during infancy. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 28, 219 – 236. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gust, N., von Fintel, R. & Petermann, F. (2017). Emotionsregulationsstrategien im Vorschulalter. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 157 – 165. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Izard, C. E., Stark, K., Trentacosta, C. & Schultz, D. (2008). Beyond emotion regulation: Emotion utilization and adaptive functioning. Child Development Perspectives, 2, 156 – 163. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Juen, F., Bänninger-Huber, E. & Peham, D. (2012). Geschlechts- und Altersunterschiede in der Emotionserkennung von Kindern und Jugendlichen. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 44, 178 – 191. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Kauschke, C., Bahn, D., Vesker, M. & Schwarzer, G. (2017). Semantische Repräsentation von Emotionsbegriffen bei Kindern im Grundschulalter. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 251 – 260. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Köckeritz, M., Klinkhammer, J. & von Salisch, M. (2010). Die Entwicklung des Emotionswissens und der behavioralen Selbstregulation bei Vorschulkindern mit und ohne Migrationshintergrund. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 59, 529 – 544. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Maassarani, R., Gosselin, P., Montembeault, P. & Gagnon, M. (2014). French speaking children’s freely produced labels for facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 5: 555. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mähler, C., Petermann, U. & Greve, W. (2017). Sozial-emotionale und kognitive Fertigkeiten als Regulationskompetenzen. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 1 – 6. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Markham, R. & Adams, K. (1992). The effect of type of task on children’s identification of facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 16, 21 – 39. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Martin, S. E., Williamson, L. R., Kurtz-Nelson, E. C. & Boekamp, J. R. (2015). Emotion understanding (and misunderstanding) in clinically referred preschoolers: The role of child language and maternal depressive symptoms. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, 24 – 37. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Morgan, J. K., Izard, C. E. & King, K. A. (2009). Construct validity of the Emotion Matching Task: Preliminary evidence for convergent and criterion validity of a new emotion knowledge measure for young children. Social Development, 19, 52 – 70. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. (2016). Sprachstandserhebungstest für Kinder im Alter zwischen 3 und 5 Jahren (SET 3 – 5). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. (2017). Emotionsregulation. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 129 – 132. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Gust, N. (2016a). Inventar zur Erfassung emotionaler Kompetenzen bei Drei- bis Sechsjährigen (EMK 3 – 6). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Gust, N. (2016b). Emotionale Kompetenzen im Vorschulalter fördern (EMK-Förderprogamm). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Wiedebusch, S. (2002). Störungen beim Erwerb emotionaler Kompetenz im Kindesalter. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 50, 1 – 28. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Wiedebusch, S. (2016). Emotionale Kompetenz bei Kindern (3., überarb. u. erw. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pons, F., Harris, P. L. & de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 127 – 152. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rieffe, C. & Wiefferink, C. H. (2017). Happy faces, sad faces: Emotion understanding in toddlers and preschoolers with language impairments. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 62, 40 – 49. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenqvist, J., Lahti-Nuuttila, P., Laasonen, M. & Korkman, M. (2014). Preschoolers’ recognition of emotional expressions: Relationships with other neurocognitive capacities. Child Neuropsychology, 20, 281 – 302. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salmon, K., Evans, I. M., Moskowitz, S., Grouden, M., Parkes, F. & Miller, E. (2013). The components of young children’s emotion knowledge: Which are enhanced by adult emotion talk? Social Development, 22, 94 – 110. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schenk, L., Bau, A.-M., Borde, T., Butler, J., Lampert, T. & Neuhauser, H., et al. (2006). Bundesgesundheitsblatt – Gesundheitsforschung – Gesundheitsschutz, 49, 853 – 860. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sette, S., Spinad, T. L. & Baumgartner, E. (2017). The relations of preschool children’s emotion knowledge and socially appropriate behaviors to peer likability. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 41, 532 – 541. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Strand, P. S., Downs, A. & Barbosa-Leiker, C. (2016). Does facial expression recognition provide a toehold for the development of emotion understanding? Developmental Psychology, 52, 1182 – 1191. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Trentacosta, C. J. & Fine, S. E. (2010). Emotion knowledge, social competence, and behavior problems in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic review. Social Development, 19, 1 – 29. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ulrich, F. & Petermann, F. (2017). Elterliche Emotionsdysregulation als Risikofaktor für die kindliche Entwicklung. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 133 – 146. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Voltmer, K. & Salisch, M. von (2017). , Three meta-analysis of children’s emotion knowledge and their school success. Learning and Individual Differences, 59, 107 – 118. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, Y., Liu, H. & Su, Y. (2014). Development of preschoolers’ emotion and false belief understanding: A longitudinal study. Social Behavior and Personality, 42, 645 – 654. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, Z., Lü, W., Zhang, H. & Surina, A. (2014). Free-labeling facial expressions and emotional situations in children aged 3 – 7 years: Developmental trajectory and a face inferiority effect. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 487 – 498. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Widen, S. C. (2016). The development of children’s concepts of emotion. In L. F. BarretM. Lewis J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp 307 – 318). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Widen, S. C & Russell, J. A. (2010). Differentiation in preschooler’s categories of emotion. Emotion, 10, 651 – 661. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar