Skip to main content
Originalia

Das hätte ich nicht essen sollen!

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1026/1616-3443.34.2.104

Zusammenfassung.Theoretischer Hintergrund: Auswirkungen gezügelten Essverhaltens auf kognitive Funktionen zeigen sich auch in Ereigniskorrelierten Potentzialen (EKPs). Fragestellung: Verändert die Rückmeldung über den Kaloriengehalt eines Preloads die Wahrnehmung von Nahrungsreizen bei nicht gezügelten und gezügelten Esserinnen? Methode: Gezügelten und nicht gezügelten Esserinnen wurden tachistoskopisch nahrungsbezogene und nicht nahrungsbezogene Worte präsentiert, dabei wurden EKPs abgeleitet. Ergebnisse: In einem frühen Stadium der Reizverarbeitung zeigte sich anhand der P2 ein Effekt bei den nicht gezügelten Esserinnen, hier führten Nahrungsworte zu geringeren Amplituden. Die P3 als Maß für eine bereits kontrollierte Verarbeitung war nur bei gezügelten Esserinnen auf Nahrungsworte größer. Die Rückmeldung eines hohen Kaloriengehalts verstärkte leicht diesen Effekt. Schlussfolgerung: Es scheint elektrophysiologisch nachweisbare Unterschiede in der Wahrnehnung von Nahrungsreizen zwischen den beiden Gruppen zu geben.


I should not have eaten that! Effects of correct and false feedback about calorie content on evoked potentials in restrainde eaters

Abstract.Background: The influences of restrained eating on cognitive functions are even seen in event-related potentials (ERPs). Objective: Does feedback about the calorie content of a “preload“ affect the perception of food stimuli in restrained eaters? Methods: Food-related and food-unrelated words were presented tachistoscopically to restrained and unrestrained eaters, while ERPs were recorded. Results: Regarding P2 it was shown that in an early stage of processing there was an effect only in unrestrained eaters, for whom food words led to smaller amplitudes. P3, an indicator of controlled processing, was enhanced by food words in restrained eaters only. Feedback of a high-caloric preload moderately enhanced this effect. Conclusions: There seem to be differences in the perception of food-related stimuli between these groups which can be shown by electrophysiological measures.

Literatur

  • Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B., Chou, C. L., Aunon, J. I. (1983). P3 and stimulus incentive value. Psychophysiology , 20, 95–101. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bernat, E., Bunce, S., Shevrin, H. (2001). Event-related brain potentials differentiate positive and negative mood adjectives during both supraliminal and subliminal visual processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology , 42, 11–34. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carretié, L., Martín-Loeches, M., Hinojosa, J. A., Mercado, F. (2001a). Emotion and attention interaction studied through event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 13, 1109–1128. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carretié, L., Mercado, F., Tapia, M., Hinojosa, J. A. (2001b). Emotion, attention, and the ‘negative bias’, studied through event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology , 41, 75–85. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, M. W., Rogers, P. J. (1993). Selective attention to food and body shape words in dieters and restrained nondieters. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 14, 515–517. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Green, M. W., Elliman, N. A., Rogers, P. J. (1995). Lack of effect of short-term fastening on cognitive function. Journal of Psychiatric Research , 29, 245–253. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hachl, P., Hempel, C., Pietrowsky, R. (2003). ERPs to stimulus identification in persons with restrained eating behavior. International Journal of Psychophysiology , 49, 111–121. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herman, C. P., Mack, D. (1975). Restrained and unrestrained eating. Journal of Personality , 43, 647–660. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Herman, C. P., Polivy, J. (1984). A boundary model for the regulation of eating. In Stunkrad, A. B. & Stellar, E. (Eds.), Eating and its disorders. New York: Raven Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hömberg, V., Grünewald, G., Grünewald-Zuberbier, E. (1980). The incentive value of stimuli and the P300 component of cerebral evoked potentials. In Kornhuber, H. H. & Decke, L. (Eds.). Motivation, motor and sensory processes of the brain: Electrical potentials, behaviour and clinical use (pp. 630-633). Amsterdam: Elsevier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Johnson, R. (1986). A triarchic model of the P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology , 23, 367–384. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jones, N., Rogers, P. J. (2003). Preoccupation, food, and failure: an investigation of cognitive performance deficits in dieters. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 33, 185–192. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kahan, D., Polivy, J., Herman, C. P. (2003). Conformity and dietary disinhibition: A test of ego-strength model of self-regulation. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 33, 165–171. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mahamedi, F., Heatherton, T. F. (1993). Effects of high caloric preloads on selective processing of food and body shape stimuli among dieters and nondieters. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 13, 305–314. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Margraf, J., Schneider, S., Ehlers, A. (1994). Diagnostisches Interview bei psychischen Störungen (2. unveränderte Auflage). Berlin: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Naumann, E., Bartussek, D., Diedrich, O., Laufer, M. E. (1992). Assessing cognitive and affective information processing functions of the brain by means of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. Journal of Psychophysiology , 6, 285–298. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Odgen, J., Greeville, L. (1993). Cognitive changes to preloading in restrained and unrestrained eaters as measured by the stroop task. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 14, 185–195. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Perpiná, C., Hemsley, D., Treasure, J., de Silva, P. (1993). Is the selective information processing of food and body words specific to patients with eating disorders?. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 14, 359–366. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Plihal, W., Haenschel, C., Hachl, P., Born, J., Pietrowsky, R. (2001). The effect of food deprivation on ERP during identification of tachistoscopically presented food-related words. Journal of Psychophysiology , 15, 163–172. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Polich, J. K., Kok, A. (1995). Cognitive and biological determinants of the P300: an integrative review. Biological Psychology , 41, 103–146. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pudel, V., Westenhöfer, J. (1989). Fragebogen zum Eßverhalten. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Pudel, V., Westenhöfer, J. (1998). Ernährungspsychologie. Eine Einführung (2. überarbeitete und erweiterte Auflage). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rogers, P. J., Green, M. W. (1993). Dieting, dietary restraint and cognitive performance. British Journal of Clinical Psychology , 32, 113–116. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: the late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology , 37, 257–261. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Silva, J. R., Pizzagalli, D. A., Larson, C. L., Jackson, D. C., Davidson, R. J. (2002). Frontal brain asymmetry in restrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology , 111, 676–681. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stewart, S. H., Samoluk, S. B. (1997). Effects of short-term food deprivation and chronic dietary restraint on the selective processing of appetitive-related cues. International Journal of Eating Disorders , 21, 129–135. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Williams, J. M. G., Healy, H., Eade, J., Windle, G., Cowen, P. J., Green, M. W., Durlach, P. (2002). Mood, eating behaviour and attention. Psychological Medicine , 32, 469–481. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar