Skip to main content
Free AccessThemenschwerpunkt

Zur Bedeutung auffälliger Exekutivfunktionen in der Diagnostik einer Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1024/1661-4747/a000359

Zusammenfassung. Defizite in Exekutivfunktionen und insbesondere in der Inhibitionsfähigkeit gelten verschiedenen Modellen zufolge als Kerndefizite einer Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS). Die Defizite sind sowohl auf einer Verhaltensebene als auch auf neurobiologischer Ebene belegt, finden aber bislang kaum Einzug in die klinische Diagnostik. Verschiedene Erhebungsverfahren werden vorgestellt und die Probleme im Bereich der klinischen Diagnostik skizziert. Viele Aufgaben messen nicht eine spezifische Exekutivfunktion, sondern umfassen immer auch andere kognitive Prozesse wie zum Beispiel Aufmerksamkeit oder Test- und Leistungsmotivation. Die Sensitivität vieler Aufgaben ist aufgrund der Heterogenität von ADHS durch verschiedene Entwicklungspfade oft nicht gewährleistet und Defizite in Exekutivfunktionen und der Inhibitionsfähigkeit sind auch nicht spezifisch für ADHS. Dennoch ist eine Diagnostik auffälliger Exekutivfunktionen und insbesondere von Defiziten in der Inhibitionsfähigkeit angebracht, da nur so Aussagen über zugrunde liegende Prozesse und Ursachen einer ADHS getroffen werden können, welche die Voraussetzung für gezielte Interventionen darstellen, wie zum Beispiel Inhibitionstrainings oder Neurofeedback.


On the Relevance of Impaired Executive Functions in the Diagnostics of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity

Abstract. Deficits in executive functions, in particular in behavioral inhibition, are at the core of several theoretical models of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). These deficits have been demonstrated on a behavioral and on a neurobiological level. However, clinical diagnostics of ADHD only rarely contain instruments to assess executive functions or behavioral inhibition. Several instruments to measure executive functions are presented and difficulties in their use are discussed. Many tasks suffer from the task impurity problem: They do not measure one specific executive function. Instead, several other cognitive (e.g., attention) and motivational processes (e.g., test motivation or achievement motivation) are involved. Due to the heterogeneity of ADHD and different developmental pathways of the disorder, measures of executive functions lack sensitivity. In similar, the specificity is compromised as well as deficits in behavioral inhibition and executive functions are not limited to ADHD. However, the clinical diagnostics of executive functions and, in particular, behavioral inhibition are recommended in order to enable assertions on impaired underlying mechanisms and processes. Such assertions are necessary to apply specific interventions, as for instance, trainings of behavioral inhibition or neurofeedback.

Literatur

  • Alegria, A. A., Wulff, M., Brinson, H., Barker, G. J., Norman, L. J., Brandeis, D.et al.(2017). Real-time fMRI neurofeedback in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 38, 3190–3209. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child Behavior Checklist. (1998). Elternfragebogen über das Verhalten von Kindern und Jugendlichen; deutsche Bearbeitung der Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL/4–18). Einführung und Anleitung zur Handauswertung (2. Aufl.). Köln: Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., Willcutt, E. G., DeFries, J. C. & Olson, R. K. (2015). Sex differences in ADHD symptom severity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56, 632–639. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Astill, R. G., Van der Heijden, K. B., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H. & Van Someren, E. J. W. (2012). Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: A century of research meta-analyzed. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1109–1138. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–423. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barkley, R. A. (2011). Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, self-regulation, and executive functioning. In K., D. VohsF., Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp.551–563). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Barkley, R. A. & Murphy, K. R. (2010). Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: The predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25, 157–173. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barkley, R. A. & Murphy, K. R. (2011). The nature of Executive Function (EF) deficits in daily life activities in adults with ADHD and their relationship to performance on EF tests. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 137–158. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Batty, M. J., Liddle, E. B., Pitiot, A., Toro, R., Groom, M. J., Scerif, G.et al.(2010). Cortical gray matter in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: a structural magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 229–238. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Baumeister, S., Wolf, I., Holz, N., Boecker-Schlier, R., Adamo, N., Holtmann, M.et al.(2016). Neurofeedback training effects on inhibitory brain activation in ADHD: A matter of learning?Neuroscience, 378, 89–99. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F.et al.(2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 757–766. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bledsoe, J. C., Xiao, D., Chaovalitwongse, A., Mehta, S., Grabowski, T. J., Semrud-Clikeman, M.et al.(2016). Diagnostic classification of ADHD versus control: Support Vector Machine classification using brief neuropsychological assessment. Journal of Attention Disorders. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/1087054716649666 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Lei, Z., Trommer, B. L., Davenport, N. D.et al.(2005). Larger deficits in brain networks for response inhibition than for visual selective attention in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 46, 94–111. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brown, T. E. (2018). ADHS bei Kindern und Erwachsenen – eine neue Sichtweise. Bern: Hogrefe. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bruchmüller, K., Margraf, J. & Schneider, S. (2012). Is ADHD diagnosed in accord with diagnostic criteria? Overdiagnosis and influence of client gender on diagnosis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 128–138. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bühner, M., Ziegler, M., Bohnes, B. & Lauterbach, K. (2006). Übungseffekte in den TAP Untertests Test Go/Nogo und Geteilte Aufmerksamkeit sowie dem Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungstest (d2). Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 17, 191–199. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Carver, C. S. & Scheier, M. F. (2011). Self-regulation of action and affect. In K.D., VohsF., Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (2nd ed., pp.3–21). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Chaytor, N., Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. & Burr, R. (2006). Improving the ecological validity of executive functioning assessment. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 217–227. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Coghill, D. R., Seth, S. & Matthews, K. (2014). A comprehensive assessment of memory, delay aversion, timing, inhibition, decision making and variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Advancing beyond the three-pathway models. Psychological Medicine, 44, 1989–2001. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Ecker, C., Giampietro, V., Taylor, E. & Rubia, K. (2010). Reduced activation and inter-regional functional connectivity of fronto-striatal networks in adults with childhood Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and persisting symptoms during tasks of motor inhibition and cognitive switching. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 44, 629–639. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Depue, B. E., Burgess, G. C., Bidwell, L. C., Willcutt, E. G. & Banich, M. T. (2010). Behavioral performance predicts grey matter reductions in the right inferior frontal gyrus in young adults with combined type ADHD. Psychiatry Research – Neuroimaging, 182, 231–237. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and Programs That Improve Children's Executive Functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 335–341. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dickstein, S. G., Bannon, K., Xavier Castellanos, F. & Milham, M. P. (2006). The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 47, 1051–1062. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Drechsler, R. & Steinhausen, H.-C. (2013). BRIEF – Verhaltensinventar zur Beurteilung Exekutiver Funktion. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Durston, S., Van Belle, J. & De Zeeuw, P. (2011). Differentiating frontostriatal and fronto-cerebellar circuits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 69, 1178–1184. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fair, D. A., Bathula, D., Nikolas, M. A. & Nigg, J. T. (2012). Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with ADHD. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 6769–6774. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Földényi, M., Giovanoli, A., Tagwerker-Neuenschwander, F.Schallberger, U. & Steinhausen, H.-C. (2000). Reliabilität und Retest-Stabilität der Testleistungen von 7–10jährigen Kindern in der computerunterstützten TAP. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 11, 1–11. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Franke, B., Neale, B. M. & Faraone, S. V. (2009). Genome-wide association studies in ADHD. Human Genetics, 126, 13–50. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawrilow, C., Schmitt, K. & Rauch, W. (2011). Kognitive Kontrolle und Selbstregulation bei Kindern mit ADHS. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 20, 41–48. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Sergeant, J. A., Verté, S.et al.(2004). How specific are executive functioning deficits in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Autism? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45, 836–854. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gmehlin, D., Stelzel, C., Weisbrod, M., Kaiser, S. & Aschenbrenner, S. (2012). Wiener Testsystem: SWITCH Task-Switching. Mödling: Schuhfried. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hampel, P., & Petermann, F. (2017). Stressverarbeitung, Lebensqualität und körperliche Beanspruchungssymptome bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 26, 58–69. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Hampel, P., Petermann, F. & Desman, C. (2009). Exekutive Funktionen bei Jungen mit Aufmerksamkeitdefizit-/Hyperraktivitätsstörung im Kindesalter. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 18, 144–152. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Hart, H., Radua, J., Nakao, T., Mataix-Cols, D. & Rubia, K. (2013). Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of inhibition and attention in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Exploring task-specific, stimulant medication, and age effects. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 185–198. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hasselhorn, M., Schumann-Hengsteler, R., Gronauer, J., Grube, D., Mähler, C., Schmid, I.et al.(2012). Arbeitsgedächtnistestbatterie für Kinder von 5 bis 12 Jahren (AGTB 5–12). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Holtmann, M., Grasmann, D., Cionek-Szpak, E., Hager, V., Panzner, N., Beyer, A.et al.(2009). Spezifische Wirksamkeit von Neurofeedback auf die Impulsivität bei ADHS. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 18, 95–104. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • van Hulst, B. M., de Zeeuw, P. & Durston, S. (2015). Distinct neuropsychological profiles within ADHD: A latent class analysis of cognitive control, reward sensitivity and timing. Psychological Medicine, 45, 735–745. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van Hulst, B. M., de Zeeuw, P., Vlaskamp, C., Rijks, Y., Zandbelt, B. B. & Durston, S. (2018). Children with ADHD symptoms show deficits in reactive but not proactive inhibition, irrespective of their formal diagnosis. Psychological Medicine. Advance onlince publication. doi:10.1017/S0033291718000107 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ikeda, Y., Okuzumi, H. & Kokubun, M. (2013). Stroop/reverse-Stroop interference in typical development and its relation to symptoms of ADHD. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 2391–2398. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobs, C. & Petermann, F. (2007). Aufmerksamkeitsstörungen bei Kindern. Langzeiteffekte des neuropsychologischen Gruppenprogrammes ATTENTIONER. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 16, 40–49. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobs, C. & Petermann, F. (2008). Aufmerksamkeitstherapie bei Kindern – Langzeiteffekte des ATTENTIONERS. Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 36, 411–417. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobs, C. & Petermann, F. (2013). Training für Kinder mit Aufmerksamkeitsstörungen. Das neuropsychologische Gruppenprogramm ATTENTIONER (3., veränd. Aufl.). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jenni, O. (2016). Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit‑/Hyperaktivitätsstörung. Monatsschrift Kinderheilkunde, 164, 271–277. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jurado, M. B. & Rosselli, M. (2007). The elusive nature of executive functions: A review of our current understanding. Neuropsychology Review, 17, 213–233. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kaiser, S., Aschenbrenner, S., Pfüller, U., Roesch-Ely, D. & Weisbrod, M. (2012). Wiener Testsystem: INHIB Response Inhibition. Mödling: Schuhfried. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Karbach, J. & Unger, K. (2014). Executive control training from middle childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 390. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Khng, K. H. & Lee, K. (2014). The relationship between stroop and stop-signal measures of inhibition in adolescents: Influences from variations in context and measure estimation. PLoS ONE, 9, e101356. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kobel, M., Bechtel, N., Weber, P., Specht, K., Klarhöfer, M., Scheffler, K.et al.(2009). Effects of methylphenidate on working memory functioning in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 13, 516–523. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kofler, M. J., Sarver, D. E., Harmon, S. L., Moltisanti, A., Aduen, P. A., Soto, E. F.et al.(2018). Working memory and organizational skills problems in ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 59, 57–67. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Koglin, U. & Petermann, F. (2004). Das Konzept der Inhibition in der Psychopathologie. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 52, 91–117. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Konrad, K. & Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). Is the ADHD brain wired differently? A review on structural and functional connectivity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 904–916. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kovshoff, H., Williams, S., Vrijens, M., Danckaerts, M., Thompson, M., Yardley, L., … Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2012). The decisions regarding ADHD management (DRAMa) study: Uncertainties and complexities in assessment, diagnosis and treatment, from the clinician's point of view. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 21, 87–99. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kray, J., Karbach, J., Haenig, S. & Freitag, C. (2012). Can task-switching training enhance executive control functioning in children with Attention Deficit/-Hyperactivity Disorder?Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1–9. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lidzba, K., Christiansen, H. & Drechsler, R. (2013). Conners Skalen zu Aufmerksamkeit und Verhalten-3. Deutschsprachige Adaptation der Conners 3rd edition von Keith Conners. Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ma, J., Lei, D., Jin, X., Du, X., Jiang, F., Li, F.et al.(2012). Compensatory brain activation in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder during a simplified Go/No-go task. Journal of Neural Transmission, 119, 613–619. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V. & Nimmo-Smith, I. (2006). Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch) (Deutsche Bearbeiteung von R. Horn und R. S. Jäger). Frankfurt: Harcourt Test Services. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Matthys, W., Vanderschuren, L. J. M. J., Schutter, D. J. L. G. & Lochman, J. E. (2012). Impaired Neurocognitive Functions Affect Social Learning Processes in Oppositional Defiant Disorder and Conduct Disorder: Implications for Interventions. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15, 234–246. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Meer, D.-J. & van der Meere, J. (2004). Response inhibition in children with conduct disorder and borderline intellectual functioning. Child Neuropsychology, 10, 189–194. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Merkt, J., Reinelt, T. & Petermann, F. (2015). A framework of psychological compensation in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1580. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Merkt, J., Siniatchkin, M. & Petermann, F. (2016). Neuropsychological measures in the diagnosis of ADHD in preschool: Can developmental research inform diagnostic practice?Journal of Attention Disorders. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1177/1087054716629741 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A. & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, a H., Howerter, A. & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Naber, M., Vedder, A., Brown, S. B. R. E. & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2016). Speed and lateral inhibition of stimulus processing contribute to individual differences in stroop-task performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 822. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E. & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal heterogeneity in Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes?Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1224–1230. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. (Hrsg.) (2010). K-CAB. Kaufman – Computerized Assessment Battery. Deutsche Adaptation des französischen Originalverfahrens K-Classic. Frankfurt: Pearson Assessment. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Petermann, F. & Toussaint, A. (2009). Neuropsychologische Diagnostik bei Kindern mit ADHS. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 18, 83–94. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Rapport, M. D., Orban, S. a., Kofler, M. J. & Friedman, L. M. (2013). Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 1237–1252. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rauch, W. A., Gold, A. & Schmitt, K. (2012). To what extent are task-switching deficits in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder independent of impaired inhibition?ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 4, 179–187. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Re, A. M., Capodieci, A. & Cornoldi, C. (2015). Effect of training focused on executive functions (attention, inhibition, and working memory) in preschoolers exhibiting ADHD symptoms. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1161. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rodewald, K., Weisbrod, M. & Aschenbrenner, S. (2004). Manual. Trail Making Test Langsteinbacher Version. Mödling: Schuhfried. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rodewald, K., M., WeisbrodS., Aschenbrenner (2012). Wiener Testsystem: Trail Making Test- Langensteinbacher Version (TMT-L). Mödling: Schuhfried. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rommelse, N. & de Zeeuw, P. (2014). Neurobiological measures to classify ADHD: A critical appraisal. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 23, 243–246. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rubia, K. (2011). “Cool” inferior frontostriatal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder versus “hot” ventromedial orbitofrontal-limbic dysfunction in conduct disorder: A review. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e69–e87. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rubia, K., Smith, A., Brammer, M., Toone, B. & Taylor, E. (2005). Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1067–1075. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Scheres, A., Oosterlaan, J. & Sergeant, J. A. (2006). Speed of inhibition predicts teacher-rated medication response in boys with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 53, 93–109. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt, S., Brücher, K. & Petermann, F. (2006). Komorbidität der Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS) im Erwachsenenalter: Perspektiven für die Diagnostik mit dem Screeningverfahren BAS-E. Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie, Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 54, 123–132. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schuhfried, G. (2012). Wiener Testsystem: STROOP Interferenztest nach Stroop. Mödling: Schuhfried. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein, D.et al.(2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19649–19654. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sonuga-Barke, E., Bitsakou, P. & Thompson, M. (2010). Beyond the dual pathway model: Evidence for the dissociation of timing, inhibitory, and delay-related impairments in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 345–355. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD – a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioural Brain Research, 130, 29–36. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1231–1238. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K.et al.(2014). Behavioral components of impulsivity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 850–886. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Subcommitee on Attenttion-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder and Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management (2011). ADHD: Clinical practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 128, 1007–1022. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Nutley, S. B., Bohlin, G. & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science, 12, 106–113. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tischler, L., Karpinski, N. & Petermann, F. (2011). Evaluation des neuropsychologischen Gruppenprogramms ATTENTIONER zur Aufmerksamkeitstherapie bei Kindern und Jugendlichen. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 22, 75–85. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Toplak, M. E., Dockstader, C. & Tannock, R. (2006). Temporal information processing in ADHD: Findings to date and new methods. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 151, 15–29. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Toplak, M. E. & Tannock, R. (2005). Time perception: Modality and duration effects in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 639–654. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Toplak, M. E., West, R. F. & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Practitioner Review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 54, 131–143. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Toussaint, A. & Petermann, F. (2010). Klinische Validität der K-CAB bei Kindern mit ADHS. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, 21, 133–141. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Verbruggen, F. & Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative learning in the Go/No-Go and Stop-Signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 649–672. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vohs, K. D. & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Understanding self-regulation. In R.F., BaumeisterK.D., Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp.1–9). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wåhlstedt, C., Thorell, L. B. & Bohlin, G. (2009). Heterogeneity in ADHD: Neuropsychological pathways, comorbidity and symptom domains. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 551–564. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wessel, J. R. (2018). Prepotent motor activity and inhibitory control demands in different variants of the go/no-go paradigm. Psychophysiology, 55. Advance online publication. doi:10.1111/psyp.12871 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V. & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the Executive Function Theory of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1336–1346. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Willoughby, M., Holochwost, S. J., Blanton, Z. E. & Blair, C. B. (2014). Executive functions: formative versus reflective measurement. Measurement, 12, 69–95. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. & the Family Life Project Investigators. (2016). Measuring executive function in early childhood: a case for formative measurement. Psychological Assessment, 8, 583–592. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Woerner, W., Becker, A. & Rothenberger, A. (2004). Normative data and scale properties of the German parent SDQ. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, ii3–ii10. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • World Health Organization. (2009). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (ICD-10). Genf: WHO. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Yasumura, A., Yamamoto, H., Yasumura, Y., Moriguchi, Y., Hiraki, K., Nakagawa, E.et al.(2015). Cognitive shifting in children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: a near infrared spectroscopy study. Journal of Psychiatry, 18, 196. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • de Zeeuw, P., Mandl, R. C. W., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., van Engeland, H. & Durston, S. (2012). Decreased frontostriatal microstructural organization in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Human Brain Mapping, 33, 1941–1951. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • de Zeeuw, P., Weusten, J., van Dijk, S., van Belle, J. & Durston, S. (2012). Deficits in cognitive control, timing and reward sensitivity appear to be dissociable in ADHD. PLoS ONE, 7, e51416. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1, 297–301. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zelazo, P. D., Blair, C. B., Willoughby, M. T., Larson, M., Officer, P., Higgins, E.et al.(2016). Executive Function: Implications for Education (NCER 2017–2000). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Research, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, P. & Fimm, B. (2017). Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP) Version 2.3.1. Herzogenrath: Psytest. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, P., Gondan, M. & Fimm, B. (2002). Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung für Kinder KITAP. Herzogenrath: Psytest. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar