Skip to main content
Article

Orienting and Focusing in Voluntary and Involuntary Visuospatial Attention Conditions

An Event-Related Potential Study

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803/a000010

In the present study, we used event-related potentials (ERPs) and behavioral measurements in a peripherally cued line-orientation discrimination task to investigate the underlying mechanisms of orienting and focusing in voluntary and involuntary attention conditions. Informative peripheral cue (75% valid) with long stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) was used in the voluntary attention condition; uninformative peripheral cue (50% valid) with short SOA was used in the involuntary attention condition. Both orienting and focusing were affected by attention type. Results for attention orienting in the voluntary attention condition confirmed the “sensory gain control theory,” as attention enhanced the amplitude of the early ERP components, P1 and N1, without latency changes. In the involuntary attention condition, compared with invalid trials, targets in the valid trials elicited larger and later contralateral P1 components, and smaller and later contralateral N1 components. Furthermore, but only in the voluntary attention condition, targets in the valid trials elicited larger N2 and P3 components than in the invalid trials. Attention focusing in the involuntary attention condition resulted in larger P1 components elicited by targets in small-cue trials compared to large-cue trials, whereas in the voluntary attention condition, larger P1 components were elicited by targets in large-cue trials than in small-cue trials. There was no interaction between orienting and focusing. These results suggest that orienting and focusing of visual-spatial attention are deployed independently regardless of attention type. In addition, the present results provide evidence of dissociation between voluntary and involuntary attention during the same task.

References

  • Anllo-Vento, L. (1995). Shifting attention in visual space: The effects of peripheral cueing on brain cortical potentials. International Journal of Neuroscience, 80, 353–370. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berger, A. , Henik, A. , Rafal, R. (2005). Competition between endogenous and exogenous orienting of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 134, 207–221. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Castiello, U. , Umiltà, C. (1990). Size of the attentional focus and efficiency of procession. Acta Psychologica, 73, 195–209. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cheal, M. , Lyon, D. R. (1991). Central and peripheral precuing of forced-choice discrimination. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, A 43, 859–880. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, V. P. , Fan, S. , Hillyard, S. A. (1995). Identification of early visual evoked potential generators by retinotopic and topographic analyses. Human Brain Mapping, 2, 170–187. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Clark, V. P. , Hillyard, S. A. (1996). Spatial selective attention affects early extrastriate but not striate components of the visual evoked potential. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 387–402. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Correa, Á. , Lupiáñez, J. , Madrid, E. , Tudela, P. (2006). Temporal attention enhances early visual procession: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1076, 116–128. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Correa, Á. , Lupiáñez, J. , Tudela, P. (2005). Attentional preparation based on temporal expectancy modulates processing at the perceptual level. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12, 328–334. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Doallo, S. , Lorenzo-Lopez, L. , Vizoso, C. , Rodriguez, H. S. , Amenedo, E. , Bara, S. , Cadaveira, F. (2004). The time course of the effects of central and peripheral cues on visual processing: An event-related potentials study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115, 199–210. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eimer, M. (1993). Spatial cueing, sensory gating, and selective response preparation: An ERP study on visuospatial orienting. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 88, 408–420. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eriksen, C. W. , St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception and Psychophysics, 40, 225–240. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eriksen, C. W. , Yeh, Y. Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 583–597. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fu, S. , Caggiano, D. M. , Greenwood, P. M. , Parasuraman, R. (2005). Event-related potentials reveal dissociable mechanisms for orienting and focusing visuospatial attention. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 341–353. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fu, S. , Fan, S. , Chen, L. , Zhuo, Y. (2001). The attentional effects of peripheral cueing as revealed by two event-related potential studies. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 172–185. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fu, S. , Zinni, M. , Squire, P. N. , Kumar, R. , Caggiano, D. M. , Parasuraman, R. (2008). When and where perceptual load interacts with voluntary visuospatial attention: An event-related potential and dipole modeling study. NeuroImage, 39, 1345–1355. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gowen, E. , Abadi, R. V. , Poliakoff, E. , Hansen, P. C. , Miall, R. C. (2007). Modulation of saccadic intrusions by exogenous and endogenous attention. Brain Research, 1141, 154–167. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwood, P. M. , Parasuraman, R. (1999). Scale of attentional focus in visual search. Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 837–859. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwood, P. M. , Parasuraman, R. (2004). The scaling of spatial attention in visual search and its modification in healthy aging. Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 3–22. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Griffin, I. C. , Miniussi, C. , Nobre, A. C. (2002). Multiple mechanisms of selective attention: Differential modulation of stimulus processing by attention to space or time. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2325–2340. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hawkins, H. L. , Hillyard, S. A. , Luck, S. J. , Mouloua, M. , Downing, C. J. (1990). Visual attention modulates signal detectability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 802–811. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hillyard, S. A. , Luck, S. J. , Mangun, G. R. (1994). The cuing of attention to visual field locations: Analysis with ERP recordings. In H. J. Heinze, T. F. Münte, G. R. Mangun (Eds.), Cognitive electrophysiology: Event-related brain potentials in basic and clinical research (pp. 1–25). Boston, MA: Birkhäuser. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopfinger, J. B. , Mangun, G. R. (1998). Reflexive attention modulates processing of visual stimuli in human extrastriate cortex. Psychological Science, 9, 441–447. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hopfinger, J. B. , West, V. M. (2006). Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing. NeuroImage, 31, 774–789. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movement. In J. B. Long, A. D. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Luck, S. J. , Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology, 31, 291–308. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Luo, Y. J. , Greenwood, P. M. , Parasuraman, R. (2001). Dynamics of the spatial scale of visual attention revealed by brain event-related potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 371–381. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mangun, G. R. (1995). Neural mechanism of visual selective attention. Psychophysiology, 32, 4–18. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mangun, G. R. , Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Electrophysiological studies of visual selective attention in humans. In A. B. Scheibel, A. F. Wechsler (Eds.), Neurobiology of higher cognitive function (pp. 271–295). New York: Guilford. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mangun, G. R. , Hillyard, S. A. (1991). Modulations of sensory-evoked brain potentials indicate changes in perceptual processing during visual-spatial priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 1057–1074. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mangun, G. R. , Hillyard, S. A. , Luck, S. J. (1993). Electrocortical substrates of visual selective attention. In D. Meyer, S. Kornblum (Eds.), Attention and performance XIV (pp. 219–243). Cambridge, MA: MIT. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Miniussi, C. , Wilding, E. , Coull, J. , Nobre, A. (1999). Orienting attention in time: Modulation of brain potentials. Brain, 122, 1507–1518. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, H. J. (1994). Qualitative differences in response bias from spatial cueing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 218–241. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Müller, H. J. , Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: Time course of activation and resistance to interruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 315–330. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Posner, M. I. , Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma, D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds.), Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 531–556). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Riggio, L. , Kirsner, K. (1997). The relationship between central cues and peripheral cues in covert visual orientation. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 885–899. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Semlitsch, H. V. , Anderer, P. , Schuster, P. , Presslich, O. (1986). A solution for reliable and valid reduction of ocular artifacts, applied to the P300 ERP. Psychophysiology, 23, 695–703. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Turatto, M. , Benso, F. , Facoetti, A. , Galfano, G. , Mascetti, G. G. , Umilta, C. (2000). Automatic and voluntary focusing of attention. Perception and Psychophysics, 62, 935–952. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vogel, E. K. , Luck, S. J. (2000). The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiology, 37, 190–203. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Woldorff, M. G. (1993). Distortion of ERP averages due to overlap from temporally adjacent ERPs: Analysis and correction. Psychophysiology, 30, 98–119. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yamaguchi, S. , Tsuchiya, H. , Kobayashi, S. (1994). Electroencephalographic activity associated with shifts of visuospatial attention. Brain, 117, 553–562. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar