Skip to main content
Original Articles and Reviews

Connected Creativity

The Impact of Web Search on Everyday Creative Thinking

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000472

Abstract. With more than 60% of the world’s population online, how does our rapidly evolving digital world impact creative processes and outcomes? On the one hand, there is the promise of the shared knowledge and ideas of humanity, readily available at our fingertips, providing numerous starting points from which to develop new ideas. On the other hand, we may be overwhelmed by the volume of information, struggle to find and identify quality information to form the basis of a creative thinking process, and instead fall back on common, accepted ideas. Throughout this article, we place creators and creating in the ubiquitous situated context of searching the World Wide Web (i.e., the Web) and consider the implications for a range of everyday creative thinking processes. Research in this area is surprisingly limited, and a number of suggestions are made to take this area forward as the Web becomes an ever-expanding part of our cognitive ecology.

References

  • Ackerman, R. (2019). Heuristic cues for meta-reasoning judgments: Review and methodology. Psihologijske Teme, 28(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.31820/pt.28.1.1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ackerman, R., Douven, I., Elqayam, S., & Teodorescu, K. (2020). Satisficing, meta-reasoning, and the rationality of further deliberation. In S. ElqayamI. DouvenJ. EvansN. CruzEds., Logic and Uncertainty in the Human Mind (pp. 10–26). Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ackerman, R., & Thompson, V. (2018). Meta-reasoning: Shedding meta-cognitive light on reasoning research. In L. BallV. ThompsonEds., The Routledge international handbook of thinking & reasoning (pp. 1–15). Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Agnoli, S., Franchin, L., Rubaltelli, E., & Corazza, G. E. (2015). An eye-tracking analysis of irrelevance processing as moderator of openness and creative performance. Creativity Research Journal, 27(2), 125–132. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2015.1030304 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ball, L. J., & Christensen, B. T. (2019). Advancing an understanding of design cognition and design metacognition: Progress and prospects. Design Studies, 65, 35–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2019.10.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barker, R. (2018). Trapped in the filter bubble? Exploring the influence of Google search on the creative process. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 18(2), 85–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/15252019.2018.1487810 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barr, N., Pennycook, G., Stolz, J. A., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). The brain in your pocket: Evidence that Smartphones are used to supplant thinking. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 473–480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.029 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beaty, R. E., & Kenett, Y. (2020). Mapping the creative mind. American Scientist, 108(4), 218–224. https://doi.org/10.1511/2020.108.4.218 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Beaty, R. E., & Silvia, P. J. (2012). Why do ideas get more creative across time? An executive interpretation of the serial order effect in divergent thinking tasks. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(4), 309–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029171 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benedek, M., & Fink, A. (2019). Toward a neurocognitive framework of creative cognition: The role of memory, attention, and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 116–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.11.002 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Benedek, M., & Jauk, E. (2018). Spontaneous and controlled processes in creative cognition. In K. ChristoffK. C. R. FoxEds., The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought (pp. 285–298). Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Bilalić, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2008). Why good thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the pernicious Einstellung (set) effect. Cognition, 108(3), 652–661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.005 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blackmon, M. H. (2012). Information scent determines attention allocation and link selection among multiple information patches on a webpage. Behaviour & Information Technology, 31(1), 3–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144929x.2011.599041 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Blair, C. S., & Mumford, M. D. (2007). Errors in idea evaluation: Preference for the unoriginal? The Journal of Creative Behavior, 41(3), 197–222. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2007.tb01288.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brand-Gruwel, S., Wopereis, I., & Walraven, A. (2009). A descriptive model of information problem solving while using internet. Computers & Education, 53(4), 1207–1217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2009.06.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Calic, G., Shamy, N. E., Kinley, I., Watter, S., & Hassanein, K. (2020). Subjective semantic surprise resulting from divided attention biases evaluations of an idea’s creativity. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 2144. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-59096-y First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carr, N. (2010, June). Does the Internet make you dumber? Wall Street Journal. https://learning.hccs.edu/faculty/tina.schwettmann/engl1301/unit-1-education-in-america-and-rhetoric/nicholas-carrs-does-the-internet-make-you-dumber/view First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2003). Decreased latent inhibition is associated with increased creative achievement in high-functioning individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 499–506. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.3.499 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cook, V., Major, L., Warwick, P., & Vrikki, M. (2020). Developing collaborative creativity through microblogging: A material-dialogic approach. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 37, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2020.100685 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Corazza, G. E. (2016). Potential originality and effectiveness: The dynamic definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 28(3), 258–267. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2016.1195627 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • DeSchryver, M. (2017). Using the Web as a higher order thinking partner: Case study of an advanced learner creatively synthesizing knowledge on the web. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 55(2), 240–271. https://doi.org/10.1177/0735633116667356 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dietrich, A. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(6), 1011–1026. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196731 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Dinet, J., Chevalier, A., & Tricot, A. (2012). Information search activity: An overview. Revue Europeenne de Psychologie Appliquee/European Review of Applied Psychology, 62(2), 49–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.erap.2012.03.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellamil, M., Dobson, C., Beeman, M., & Christoff, K. (2012). Evaluative and generative modes of thought during the creative process. NeuroImage, 59(2), 1783–1794. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ellis, D. A. (2019). Are smartphones really that bad? Improving the psychological measurement of technology-related behaviors. Computers in Human Behavior, 97, 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.03.006 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive-experiential and analytical-rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390–405. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fink, A., Grabner, R. H., Gebauer, D., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., & Ebner, F. (2010). Enhancing creativity by means of cognitive stimulation: Evidence from an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1687–1695. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.072 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Benedek, M., Reishofer, G., Ischebeck, A., Weiss, E. M., & Ebner, F. (2012). Stimulating creativity via the exposure to other people’s ideas. Human Brain Mapping, 33(11), 2603–2610. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21387 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research and applications. MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Firth, J. A., Torous, J., & Firth, J. (2020). Exploring the impact of internet use on memory and attention processes. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(24), Article 9481. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17249481 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. The American Psychologist, 34(10), 906–911. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.34.10.906 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gabora, L. (2017). Honing theory: A complex systems framework for creativity. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 21(1), 35–88. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gabora, L., & Kaufman, S. (2010). Evolutionary approaches to creativity. In J. KaufmanR. SternbergEds., The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 279–300). Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Gambetti, E., Marinello, F., Zucchelli, M. M., Nori, R., & Giusberti, F. (2020). Fast thoughts and metacognitive feelings: The role of cognitive styles. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 34, 504–514. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2225 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • George, T., & Wiley, J. (2020). Need something different? Here’s what’s been done: Effects of examples and task instructions on creative idea generation. Memory & Cognition, 48(2), 226–243. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-01005-4 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • George, T., Wiley, J., Koppel, R. H., & Storm, B. C. (2019). Constraining or constructive? The effects of examples on idea novelty. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 53(3), 396–403. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.178 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., & Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: Strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. British Journal of Psychology, 98(4), 611–625. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2007.tb00467.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Glaveanu, V. P., & Beghetto, R. A. (2020). Creative experience: A non-standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 33(2), 75–80. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2020.1827606 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gocłowska, M. A., Baas, M., Crisp, R. J., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2014). Whether social schema violations help or hurt creativity depends on need for structure. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(8), 959–971. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214533132 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gonçalves, M., Cardoso, C., & Badke-Schaub, P. (2016). Inspiration choices that matter: The selection of external stimuli during ideation. Design Science, 2(e10), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1017/dsj.2016.10 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. The American Psychologist, 5(9), 444–454. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0063487 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Guilford, J. P. (1956). The structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin, 53(4), 267–293. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040755 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Hahnel, C., Goldhammer, F., Kröhne, U., & Naumann, J. (2018). The role of reading skills in the evaluation of online information gathered from search engine environments. Computers in Human Behavior, 78, 223–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Haider, J., & Sundin, O. (2019). Invisible search and online search engines: The ubiquity of search in everyday life. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429448546 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Harms, M., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Derrick, D. C. (2018). The role of information search in creative problem solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14(3), 367–380. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000212 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heersmink, R. (2016). The internet, cognitive enhancement, and the values of cognition. Minds and Machines, 26(4), 389–407. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-016-9404-3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Heersmink, R., & Sutton, J. (2020). Cognition and the web: Extended, transactive, or scaffolded? Erkenntnis, 85(1), 139–164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10670-018-0022-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kammerer, Y., Brand-Gruwel, S., & Jarodzka, H. (2018). The future of learning by searching the web: Mobile, social, and multimodal. Frontline Learning Research, 6(2), 81–91. https://doi.org/10.14786/flr.v6i2.343 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kantosalo, A., & Toivenen, H. (2016). Modes for creative human-computer collaboration: Alternating and task-divided co-creativity. Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Computational Creativity. http://www.computationalcreativity.net/iccc2016/wp-content/uploads/2016/01/Modes-for-Creative-Human-Computer-Collaboration.pdf First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kauer, A. R., & Sowden, P. T. (2020). The role of attention in the development of creativity. In K. Cohen-KadoshEd., The Oxford handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198827474.013.15 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Khaleel, I., Wimmer, B. C., Peterson, G. M., Zaidi, S. T. R., Roehrer, E., Cummings, E., & Lee, K. (2020). Health information overload among health consumers: A scoping review. Patient Education and Counseling, 103(1), 15–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pec.2019.08.008 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Kules, B. (2005). Supporting creativity with search tools. Creativity Support Tools, 50, 53–64. https://www.cs.umd.edu/hcil/CST/Papers/supportingcreativity.htm First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Laukkonen, R., Webb, M. E., Salvi, C., Tangen, J. M., & Schooler, J. (2018). Eureka heuristics: How feelings of insight signal the quality of a new idea. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ez3tn First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Liu, J., Kim, C. S., & Creel, C. (2015). Exploring search task difficulty reasons in different task types and user knowledge groups. Information Processing & Management, 51(3), 273–285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2014.10.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Literat, I., & Glăveanu, V. P. (2016). Same but different? Distributed creativity in the Internet age. Creativity: Theories – Research – Applications, 3(2), 330–342. https://doi.org/10.1515/ctra-2016-0020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Literat, I., & Glăveanu, V. P. (2018). Distributed creativity on the internet: A theoretical foundation for online creative participation. International Journal of Communication, 12, 893–908. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Lubart, T. I. (2001). Models of the creative process: Past, present and future. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), 295–308. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1334_07 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lubart, T. (Ed.). (2018). The creative process: Perspectives from multiple domains (1st ed.). Palgrave Macmillan UK. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lugli, L., Ragni, M., Piccardi, L., & Nori, R. (2017). Hypermedia navigation: Differences between spatial cognitive styles. Computers in Human Behavior, 66, 191–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.038 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Marsh, E. J., & Rajaram, S. (2019). The digital expansion of the mind: Implications of internet usage for memory and cognition. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.11.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69(3), 220–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048850 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mumford, M. D., Baughman, W. A., Threlfall, K. V., Supinski, E. P., & Costanza, D. P. (1996). Process-based measures of creative problem-solving skills: I. Problem construction. Creativity Research Journal, 9(1), 63–76. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj0901_6 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mumford, M. D., Medeiros, K. E., & Partlow, P. J. (2012). Creative thinking: Processes, strategies, and knowledge. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 46(1), 30–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419109534380 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Mumford, M. D., Mobley, M. I., Reiter-Palmon, R., Uhlman, C. E., & Doares, L. M. (1991). Process analytic models of creative capacities. Creativity Research Journal, 4(2), 91–122. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419109534380 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nestojko, J. F., Finley, J. R., Roediger, H. L., & II, I. (2013). Extending cognition to external agents. Psychological Inquiry, 24(4), 321–325. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840x.2013.844056 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Rietzschel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21(1), 34–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463281003765323 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nijstad, B. A., Stroebe, W., & Lodewijkx, H. F. M. (2002). Cognitive stimulation and interference in groups: Exposure effects in an idea generation task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(6), 535–544. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-1031(02)00500-0 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nikolov, D., Lalmas, M., Flammini, A., & Menczer, F. (2019). Quantifying biases in online information exposure. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology, 70(3), 218–229. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.24121 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Nori, R., Palmiero, M., Giusberti, F., Gambetti, E., & Piccardi, L. (2020). Web searching and navigation: Age, intelligence, and familiarity. Journal of the Association for Information Science and Technology, 71(8), 902–915. https://doi.org/10.1002/asi.24314 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ong, K., Järvelin, K., Sanderson, M., & Scholer, F. (2017). Using information scent to understand mobile and desktop Web search behavior. Proceedings of the 40th International ACM SIGIR Conference on Research and Development in Information. Retrieval – SIGIR “17, https://doi.org/10.1145/3077136.3080817 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Palani, S., Ding, Z., MacNeil, S., & Dow, S. (2021). The “active search” hypothesis: How search strategies relate to creative learning. Proceedings of the 2021 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval (CHIIR “21), 325–329. https://doi.org/10.1145/3406522.3446046 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pirolli, P. (2005). Rational analyses of information foraging on the web. Cognitive Science, 29(3), 343–373. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_20 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pirolli, P., & Card, S. (1999). Information foraging. Psychological Review, 106(4), 643–675. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.106.4.643 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologists? Potentials, pitfalls, and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist, 39(2), 83–96. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3902_1 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pringle, A., & Sowden, P. T. (2017a). The Mode Shifting Index (MSI): A new measure of the creative thinking skill of shifting between associative and analytic thinking. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 23, 17–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.10.010 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pringle, A., & Sowden, P. T. (2017b). Unearthing the creative thinking process: Fresh insights from a think-aloud study of garden design. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(3), 344–358. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000144 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Puente-Díaz, R., Cavazos-Arroyo, J., & Vargas-Barrera, F. (2021). Metacognitive feelings as a source of information in the evaluation and selection of creative ideas. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 39, Article 100767. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2020.100767 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reiter-Palmon, R. (2017). The role of problem construction in creative production. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 51(4), 323–326. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.202 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Reiter-Palmon, R., Mumford, M. D., O’Connor Boes, J., & Runco, M. A. (1997). Problem construction and creativity: The role of ability, cue consistency, and active processing. Creativity Research Journal, 10(1), 9–23. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1001_2 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rieh, S. Y., Kim, Y.-M., & Markey, K. (2012). Amount of invested mental effort (AIME) in online searching. Information Processing & Management, 48(6), 1136–1150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ipm.2012.05.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rietzschel, E. F., Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2007). Relative accessibility of domain knowledge and creativity: The effects of knowledge activation on the quantity and originality of generated ideas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(6), 933–946. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.014 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Risko, E. F., Ferguson, A. M., & McLean, D. (2016). On retrieving information from external knowledge stores: Feeling-of-findability, feeling-of-knowing and Internet search. Computers in Human Behavior, 65, 534–543. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.046 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ritter, S. M., Damian, R. I., Simonton, D. K., van Baaren, R. B., Strick, M., Derks, J., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). Diversifying experiences enhance cognitive flexibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 961–964. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.009 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 92–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.650092 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Salim, S. (2019, February 4). More than six hours of our day is spent online – Digital 2019 reports. Digitalinformationworld.Com https://www.digitalinformationworld.com/2019/02/internet-users-spend-more-than-a-quarter-of-their-lives-online.html First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Salmerón, L., Naumann, J., García, V., & Fajardo, I. (2017). Scanning and deep processing of information in hypertext: An eye tracking and cued retrospective think-aloud study. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 33(3), 222–233. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12152 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Savolainen, R. (2018). Berrypicking and information foraging: Comparison of two theoretical frameworks for studying exploratory search. Journal of Information Science, 44(5), 580–593. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165551517713168 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sawyer, K. (2012). Explaining creativity: The science of human innovation. Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sharit, J., Hernández, M. A., Czaja, S. J., & Pirolli, P. (2008). Investigating the roles of knowledge and cognitive abilities in older adult information seeking on the Web. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 15(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1145/1352782.1352785 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Simon, H. A. (1979). Models of thought. Yale University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Smart, P., Heersmink, R., & Clowes, R. W. (2017). The cognitive ecology of the internet. Cognition beyond the brain (pp. 251–282). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-49115-8_13 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, C. L., & Rieh, S. Y. (2019). Knowledge-context in search systems: Toward information-literate actions. Proceedings of the 2019 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval, 55–62. https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/ First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Schumacher, J. S. (1993). Constraining effects of examples in a creative generation task. Memory & Cognition, 21(6), 837–845. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03202751 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sowden, P. T., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning, 21(1), 40–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2014.885464 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stone, S. M., & Storm, B. C. (2019). Search fluency as a misleading measure of memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47(1), 53–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000806 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Storm, B. C. (2019). Thoughts on the digital expansion of the mind and the effects of using the internet on memory and cognition. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(1), 29–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.12.003 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sundin, O., Haider, J., Andersson, C., Carlsson, H., & Kjellberg, S. (2017). The search-ification of everyday life and the mundane-ification of search. The Journal of Documentation, 73(2), 224–243. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Swar, B., Hameed, T., & Reychav, I. (2017). Information overload, psychological ill-being, and behavioral intention to continue online healthcare information search. Computers in Human Behavior, 70, 416–425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.068 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Taramigkou, M., Apostolou, D., & Mentzas, G. (2017). Supporting creativity through the interactive exploratory search paradigm. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 33(2), 94–114. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2016.1220104 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thompson, V. A., Prowse Turner, J. A., & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63(3), 107–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.06.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Thompson, V. A., Turner, J. A. P., Pennycook, G., Ball, L. J., Brack, H., Ophir, Y., & Ackerman, R. (2013). The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency as metacognitive cues for initiating analytic thinking. Cognition, 128(2), 237–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.012 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Van Dijk, M., Blom, E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2020). The influence of situational cues on children’s creativity in an alternative uses task and the moderating effect of selective attention. Journal of Intelligence, 8(4), 37–57. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence8040037 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(1), 57–59. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014781 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Vasconcelos, L. A., & Crilly, N. (2015). Inspiration and fixation: Questions, methods, findings, and challenges. Design Studies, 42, 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2015.11.001 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. Watts & Co. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wang, S., & Thompson, V. (2019). Fluency and feeling of rightness: The effect of anchoring and models. Psihologijske Teme, 28(1), 37–72. https://doi.org/10.31820/pt.28.1.3 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • White, R., & Roth, R. (2013). Exploratory search: Beyond the query-response paradigm. Morgan & Claypool. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Wiley, J. (1998). Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 26(4), 716–730. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03211392 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Wirth, W., Böcking, T., Karnowski, V., & von Pape, T. (2007). Heuristic and systematic use of search engines. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12(3), 778–800. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2007.00350.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Xu, X., & Pang, W. (2020). Can concept mapping facilitate verbal divergent thinking? Creativity Research Journal, 32(4), 344–356. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2020.1843124 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Yamashiro, J. K., & Roediger, H. L. III (2019). Expanding cognition: A brief consideration of technological advances over the past 4000 years. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 8(1), 15–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.12.004 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zabelina, D. L. (2018). Attention and creativity. In R. E. JungO. VartanianEds., The Cambridge handbook of the neuroscience of creativity (pp. 161–179). Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(3), 136–143. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017379 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zenasni, F., Besançon, M., & Lubart, T. (2008). Creativity and tolerance of ambiguity: An empirical study. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 42(1), 61–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2008.tb01080.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhang, Y., & Capra, R. (2019). Understanding how people use search to support their everyday creative tasks. Proceedings of the 2019 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval. https://doi.org/10.1145/3295750.3298936 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zhang, Y., Capra, R., & Li, Y. (2020). An in-situ study of information needs in design-related creative projects. Proceedings of the 2020 Conference on Human Information Interaction and Retrieval, https://doi.org/10.1145/3343413.3377973 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zmigrod, S., Zmigrod, L., & Hommel, B. (2019). The relevance of the irrelevant: Attentional distractor-response binding predicts performance in the remote associates task. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13(1), 15–23. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000162 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar