Evidence of Different Developmental Trajectories for Length Estimation According to Egocentric and Allocentric Viewpoints in Children and Adults
Abstract
This study investigated the influence of egocentric and allocentric viewpoints on a comparison task of length estimation in children and adults. A total of 100 participants ranging in age from 5 years to adulthood were presented with virtual scenes representing a park landscape with two paths, one straight and one serpentine. Scenes were presented either from an egocentric or allocentric viewpoint. Results showed that when the two paths had the same length, participants always overestimated the length of the straight line for allocentric trials, whereas a development from a systematic overestimation in children to an underestimation of the straight line length in adults was found for egocentric trials. We discuss these findings in terms of the influences of both bias-inhibition processes and school acquisitions.
References
1978). Development of spatial orientation in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 14, 224–234.
(2002). The brains sense of movement (perspectives in Cognitive Neuroscience). Harvard University Press.
(2006). Emotion and reason: The cognitive neuroscience of decision making. USA: Oxford University Press.
(1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: Diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 7, 93–130.
(2006). Spatial memory: How egocentric and allocentric combine? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 551–557.
(2004). Reference frames for spatial cognition: Different brain areas are involved in viewer-, object-, and landmark-centered judgments about object location. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1517–1535.
(2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4–13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2037–2078.
(1995). Interference and inhibition in cognition. New York: Academic Press.
(2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318, 1387–1388.
(2005). Not quite as grown-up as we like to think. Psychological Science, 16, 291–297.
(2000). Inhibition and cognitive development: Object, number, categorization, and reasoning. Cognitive Development, 15, 63–73.
(2001). Negative priming effect after inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task. Developmental Science, 4, 119–123.
(2009). FMRI study of Piagetian cognitive stages in human development: A neo-Piagetian approach. 15th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, June 18th–23rd, San Francisco, USA.
(2006). Spatial knowledge acquisition from direct experience in the environment: Individual differences in the development of metric knowledge and the integration of separately learned places. Cognitive Psychology, 52, 93–129.
(2006). Cognitive inhibition of number/length interference in a Piaget-like task: Evidence from ERP and fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 498–509.
(2009). Adult brains don’t fully overcome biases that lead to incorrect performance during cognitive development: An fMRI study in young adults completing a Piaget-like task. Developmental Science, 12, 326–338.
(2008). Traveled distances: New insights into the role of optic flow. Vision Research, 48, 289–303.
(2004). The spatial brain. Neuropsychology, 18, 473–476.
(2006). Differential developmental trajectories for egocentric, environmental and intrinsic frames of reference in spatial memory. Cognition, 101, 153–172.
(1995). Multiple cues for three-dimensional shape. In , The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 351–364). Cambridge, MA, London, England: MIT Press.
(1984). Piaget’s theory. In , Handbook of child Psychology. New York: Wiley.
(1967). The Child’s conception of space. New York: Norton (First Published 1948).
(1960). The Child’s conception of geometry. . New York: W. W. Norton & Company.
(1999). La valeur de la science. France: Flammarion (originally published in 1905).
(2010). Neural bases of topographical representation in humans: Contribution of neuroimaging studies. In , The Neurophysiological foundations of mental and motor imagery (pp. 17–30). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(1979). Motion parallax as an independent cue for depth perception. Perception, 8, 125–134.
(2007). Negative priming effect after inhibition of weight/number interference in a Piaget-like task. Cognitive Development, 22, 124–129.
(2009). Preference for impossible figures in 4-month-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 115–123.
(2007). Discrimination of possible and impossible objects in infancy. Psychological Science, 18, 303–307.
(2000). The impact of home computer use on children’s activities and development. The Future of Children, 10, 123–144.
(2004). The contributions of static visual cues, nonvisual cues, and optic flow in distance estimation. Perception, 33, 49–65.
(